簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 王月慧
論文名稱: 學步兒的心智理解與母親的心智言談之相關研究
Toddlers’ Comprehension of Theory of Mind and the Effect of Mothers’ Mental State Discourse
指導教授: 張鑑如
學位類別: 博士
Doctor
系所名稱: 人類發展與家庭學系
Department of Human Development and Family Studies
論文出版年: 2014
畢業學年度: 102
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 181
中文關鍵詞: 心智理論心智言談親子共讀
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:261下載:0
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 本研究以探討學步兒心智理解發展及母親心智言談對學步兒心智理解發展的影響為目的,採方便取樣(convenience sampling)的方式於大台北地區找尋家中有16個月、18個月、21個月、24個月大學步兒的家庭,並以學步兒及其母親為研究對象。測驗學步兒心智理解能力的工具為Southgate等人(2007)設計的「預期注視」典範心智理解作業;評估學步兒語言能力的工具為「華語嬰幼兒溝通發展問卷(台灣版)」(劉惠美、曹峰銘編製,2010);調查母親心智言談的範圍包括母親與學步兒在親子共讀中產出的知覺言談、意圖/欲求言談、認知言談、及情緒言談。
    本研究包含兩個部份,第一部份為「學步兒的心智理解發展情形」研究,第二部份為「母親的心智言談與學步兒心智理解發展的關係」研究。本研究第一部份旨在檢視台灣學步兒在心智理解作業上的通過情形,以79名學步兒為對象,分四個月齡組(16個月、18個月、21個月、24個月)進行施測,施測工具為「預期注視」典範的未預期移位作業,並考驗心智理解得分在學步兒背景變項上的差異情形,同時探討學步兒心智理解發展在三個月後的變化。研究的主要結果如下:
    一、 台灣學步兒在錯誤相信理解作業上的通過率隨月齡增加而提高,學步兒於21個月大時是發展理解錯誤相信能力的重要轉銜點,挑戰了以往認定學步兒尚未發展錯誤相信理解能力的論點。
    二、 台灣學步兒的心智理解發展受社會文化的影響,在結合真實相信理解與錯誤相信理解的心智理解作業之通過率低於英語系國家,說明了學步兒心智理解發展不具普世性的特點。
    三、 在影響學步兒心智理解發展的因素方面,本研究發現學步兒的語言能力及母親教育程度對T1、T2的學步兒心智理解能力均有影響,家庭經濟地位僅在T1對心智理解能力有影響,卻未持續到T2,這顯示語言高分組的學步兒與高學歷母親的學步兒在T1、T2皆出現穩定通過心智理解作業的現象,呼應過去研究指稱高學歷母親提供的資源較為豐富多元,因而影響學步兒較早發展出理解真實相信與錯誤相信的心智理解能力,這同時顯示學步兒是在所處的社會文化情境中發展心智理解能力。

    本研究第二部份旨在探討親子共讀時母親的心智言談對學步兒心智理解的影響,研究同樣以第一部份的79名學步兒為對象,進行親子共讀的錄影與語料分析,藉以瞭解親子共讀時心智言談的內涵與學步兒心智理解得分的相關。研究的主要發現如下:
    一、 台灣母親與學步兒進行親子共讀的心智言談內容以知覺言談最多,其次是意圖/欲求言談與認知言談。國外研究發現母親最常產出的情緒言談並非台灣母親心智言談的主要內容。同時,學步兒月齡是影響台灣母親選擇心智言談類型的重要因素。
    二、 學步兒進行親子共讀的心智言談內容以知覺言談、意圖/欲求言談最多,但學步兒即使產出心智言談通常也只是點到為止,仍依賴母親的引導與敘說。因此,母親如欲鷹架學步兒的心智理解發展,便需要學習使用能促進學步兒心智理解發展的心智言談型態。
    三、 並非所有的心智言談都有助於學步兒的心智理解發展,唯有在親子共讀情境中談論個體認知、情緒狀態的心智言談才對學步兒心智理解能力有所助益,這些心智言談也是教育上提升學步兒心智理解發展的重要介入點。
    四、 在親子共讀情境中,學步兒會對自身與故事人物的心智狀態感到好奇,當學步兒有能力表達自身或故事人物的認知狀態與知覺狀態時,學步兒將從中獲取心智理解的明顯進展。

    摘 要---------------------------------------------------iii 目 次----------------------------------------------------vi 表 次--------------------------------------------------viii 圖 次----------------------------------------------------xi 第一章 緒論---------------------------------------------01 第一節 研究動機-------------------------------------------01 第二節 名詞釋義-------------------------------------------12 第三節 研究目的與研究問題----------------------------------14 第二章 文獻探討-----------------------------------------16 第一節 學步兒心智理解的發展--------------------------------17 第二節 學步兒心智理解的發展機制-----------------------------27 第三節 研究學步兒心智理解發展的方法-------------------------43 第四節 母親心智言談與學步兒心智理解的關係--------------------62 第三章 研究方法-----------------------------------------73 第一節 研究設計-------------------------------------------74 第二節 研究對象-------------------------------------------79 第三節 實施程序-------------------------------------------85 第四節 研究工具-------------------------------------------87 第五節 資料處理-------------------------------------------101 第四章 研究結果與討論------------------------------------103 第一部份 學步兒心智理解發展與相關因素 第一節 學步兒心智理解作業的通過情形-------------------------103 第二節 學步兒心智理解的追蹤發展狀況-------------------------125 第二部份 母親的心智言談與學步兒心智理解之關係 第一節 親子共讀時的心智言談--------------------------------132 第二節 心智言談型態與學步兒心智理解的關係--------------------146 第五章 結論與建議---------------------------------------155 第一節 結論----------------------------------------------155 第二節 建議----------------------------------------------159 參考書目-------------------------------------------------161 附錄一---------------------------------------------------170 附錄二---------------------------------------------------171

    中文部份
    保心怡(2003)。幼兒對於心智理解相關作業的理解情形及運用故事活動介入的效果研究。國立台灣師範大學人類發展與家庭學系博士論文,未出版,台北市。
    周育如與張鑑如(2008)。親子共讀對幼兒敘說故事主角心智狀態的影響效果。教育心理學報,40(2),261-282。
    姜忠信與張閔翔(2008)。幼兒理解個體意圖的能力:語言及表情的線索。中華心理學刊,50(3),241-256。
    張靜文(2006)。學步兒Roye的心智日記。人類發展與家庭學報,8,30-44。
    張鑑如(2002)。推動親子共讀之研究。國民教育,42,43-49。
    劉惠美、曹峰銘(2010)。華語嬰幼兒溝通發展量表(台灣版)。台北:心理。

    英文部份
    Akhtar, N., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The role of discourse novelty in early word learning. Child Development, 67, 635-645.
    Astington, J.W., Harris, P. L., & Olson, D. R. (Eds.). (1988). Developing theories of mind. New York: Cardridge University Press.
    Astington, J.W., & Jenkins, J.M. (1995). Theory of mind development and social understanding. Cognition & Emotion, 9(2-3), 151-165.
    Astington, J.W. (1998). Theory of mind, Humpty Dumpty, and the icebox. Human Development, 41, 30-9.
    Astington, J. W. (2008). More manifesto than blueprint: Katherine Nelson’s vision for the future of cognitive development research. (Essay review of “Young minds in social worlds” K. Nelson), Cognitive Development, 23, 438-444.
    Baldwin, D. (1991). Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child
    Development, 62, 875-890.
    Barker, R. (2001). The Mighty Toddler: The essential guide to the toddler years. Sydney: M. Evans.
    Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a
    'theory of mind'? Cognition, 21, 37-46.
    Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive
    mechanisms in mindreading. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 13(5),
    513-552.
    Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind.
    Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Birch, S. A. J. & Bloom, P. (2004). Understanding children's and adults' limitations in
    reasoning about the mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 255-260.
    Birch, S. A. J., & Bloom, P. (2007). The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false
    beliefs. Psychological Science, 18, 382-386.
    Bloom, P., & German, T.P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77, B25-B31.
    Bretherton, I., McNew, S., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural and verbal communication: When do infants acquire a "theory of mind?" In Lamb M. & Sherrod L. (Eds.), Infant social cognition. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum, 333-373.
    Brown, J., Donelan-McCall, N., & Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk about mental states? The significance of children's conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers. Child Development, 67(3), 836-849.
    Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
    Camaioni, L. (2001). Early language. In Bremner G. & Fogel A. (Eds), Handbook of Infant Development, Blackwell: Oxford, 404–426.
    Carlson, S.M., Moses, L.J., & Hix, H. (1998). The role of inhibitory processes in young children’s difficulties with deception and false belief. Child Development, 69, 672-691.
    Carlson, S.M. & Moses, L.J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032-1053.
    Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen- to 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315-330.
    Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1-143.
    Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive
    Development, 9, 377-395.
    Coltheart, M. (1999). Modularity and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(3), 115-120
    Corkum, V.,& Moore, C. (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In Moore C. & Dunham P. (Eds.), Joint attention: its origins and role in development, New Jersey: Hillsdale, 61-84.
    Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (1998). The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis. Developmental Science, 1, 255-259.
    Degotardi, S., & Torr, J. (2007). A longitudinal investigation of mothers’ mind-related talk to their 12- to 24-month-old infants. Early Child Development and Care, 177(6-7), 767-780.
    Denham, S., Blair, K., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., et al. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence? Child Development, 74(1), 238-256.
    Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling states and children's later understanding of others' emotions. Developmental Psychology, 27, 448-455.
    Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, 1352-1366.
    Dunn, J. & Hughes, C. (1998). Young children's understanding of emotions within
    close relationships. Cognition and Emotion, 12 (2), 171-190.
    Fabricius, V., & Imbens-Bailey, L. (2000). False beliefs about false beliefs. In P. Mitchell and K. Riggs (Ed.), Children’s reasoning about the mind, Hove: Psychology, 267-280.
    Fabricius, W.V., & Khalil, S.L. (2003). False beliefs or false positives? Limits on children’s understanding of mental representation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 239-262.
    Fabricius, W.V., T. Boyer, A.A. Weimer, & K. Carroll. (2010). True or false: Do five-year-olds understand belief? Developmental Psychology, 46, 1402-1416.
    Flavell, J.H. (1978). Metacognitive development. In J. Scandura & C. Brainerd (Eds.), Structural/process theories of complex human behavior. Netherlands: Sijthoff and Noordhoff, 213-224.
    Flavell, J.H., Shipstead, S.G., & Croft, K. (1978). Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Hiding objects from others. Child Development, 49, 1208-1211 .
    Flavell, J.H. (1986). The development of children’s knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. American Psychologist, 41, 418-425.
    Flavell, J. H., Green, E L., & Flavell, E. R. (1998). The mind has a mind of its own: Developing knowledge about mental uncontrollability. Cognitive Development, 13(1), 127-138.
    Flavell, J.H. (2000). Development of children's knowledge about the mental world. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(1), 15-23.
    Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Freeman, N.H. & Lacohée, H. (1995). Making explicit three-year-olds’ implicit competence with their own false beliefs. Cognition, 56, 31-60.
    Frye D., & Moore, C. (1991). Children's theories of mind: mental states and social understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Garnham, W. A., & Ruffman, T.(2001). Doesn’t see, doesn’t know: Is anticipatory looking really related to understanding of belief? Developmental Science, 4(1), 94-100.
    Gopnik, A. & Wellman, H.M. (1992). Why the child's theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language, 7 (1-2), 145-71.
    Gopnik, A. & Wellman, H.M. (1994). The "theory theory". In L. Hirschfield and S. Gelman (Eds.), Domain specificity in culture and cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press, 257-293.
    Hala, S. & Carpendale, J. (1997). All in the mind: Children's understanding of mental life. In S. Hala (Ed.), The development of social cognition. Hove: Psychology, 89-231.
    Hedger, J. A., & Fabricius, W. V. (2011). True belief belies false belief: Recent findings of competence in infants and limitations in 5-Year-Olds, and implications for theory of mind development. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2 (3), 429-447.
    Hogrefe, G.J., Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1986). Ignorance versus false belief: A developmental lag in attribution of epistemic states. Child Development, 57, 567-582.
    Hornick, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937-944.
    Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1997). “Pretend you didn't know”: Preschoolers' talk about
    mental states in pretend play. Cognitive Development, 12, 477-499 .
    Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1998). Understanding mind and emotion: longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 1026-1037.
    Hughes, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. Social Development, 13, 590-619.
    Jenkins, J. M., Turrell, S. L., Kogushi, Y., Lollis, S., & Ross, H. S. (2003). A longitudinal investigation of the dynamics of mental state talk in families. Child Development, 74(3), 905-920.
    Knudsen, B., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). Eighteen- and 24-month-old infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes. Developmental Science, 15, 113-122.
    Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 1830-1834.
    Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: the origins of "theory of mind".
    Psychological Review, 94, 412-426.
    Leslie, A. M. (2005). Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 459-462.
    Lillard, A. S. (1998). Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theory of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 3-33.
    Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardif, T., &Sabbagh, M. A. (2008). Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across languages and cultures. Developmental Psychology, 44, 523-531.
    Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? Cognition, 105, 489-512.
    Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A., Laakso, M. (1997). Language and symbolic play in toddlers. International Journal of Behavioral development, 21(2), 289-302.
    McElwain, N.L., Booth-LaForce, C, Wu, X. (2011).Infant-mother attachment and children's friendship quality: maternal mental-state talk as an intervening mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1295-311.
    Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (1999). Linguistic acquisitional style and mentalising development: The role of maternalmind-mindedness. Cognitive Development, 14, 363-380.
    Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73(6), 1715-1726.
    Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2003). Pathways to understanding mind: Construct validity and predictive validity of maternal mind-mindedness. Child Development,74, 1194-1211.
    Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R. and de Rosnay, M. (2013). Mind-mindedness and theory of mind: mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Development, 84(2), 89-103.
    Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: reenactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838-850.
    Meltzoff, A. (2005). Imitation and other minds: The "Like Me" hypothesis. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectiveson imitation: From cognitive neuroscience to social science. Cambridge: MIT Press, 55-77.
    Meltzoff, A., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 335-366.
    Meristo, M., Morgan, G., Geraci, A., Iozzi, L., Hjelmquist, E., Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2012). Belief attribution in deaf and hearing infants. Developmental Science, 15(5), 633-640.
    Milligan, K., Astington, J. W. and Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: meta-analysis of the relation between lLanguage ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622-646.
    Neumann, A., Thoermer, C., & Sodian, B. (2008). False belief understanding in 18-month-olds’ anticipatory looking behaviour: An eye-tracking study. Paper presented at the XXIX International Congress of Psychology, Berlin, Germany.
    Moore, C. (1996). Theories of mind in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 19-40.
    Olson, J.&Masur, F. (2011). Infants' gesturesinfluencemothers' provisionofobject, action and internal state labels. Journal of Child Language, 38(5), 1028-1054.
    O’Neill, D.K. (1996). Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to parent’s knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659-677.
    Onishi, K.H. , & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science,308, 255–258.
    Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT
    Press.
    Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (2005). Infants’ insight into the mind: How deep? Science,
    308, 214-216.
    Peterson, C., & Slaughter, V. (2003). Opening windows into the mind: Mothers' preferences for mental state explanations and children's theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 18, 399-429.
    Pratt, C. & Bryant, P. (1990). Young children understand that looking leads to knowing (so long as they are looking into a single barrel). Child Development, 61, 946-961.
    Premack, D. G. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515-526.
    Poulin-Dubois, D., Sodian, B., Metz, U., Tilden, J., & Schoeppner, B. (2007). Out of sight is not out of mind: Developmental changes in infants’ understanding of visual perception during the second year. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 401-421.
    Randell, A. C., & Peterson, C. C. (2009). Affective qualities of sibling disputes, mothers' conflict attitudes, and children's theory of mind development. Social Development, 18(4), 857-874.
    Racine, T.P., Carpendale, J., & Turnbull, W. (2006). Cross-sectional and longitudinal relations between mother-child talk about conflict and children's social understanding. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 521-536.
    Rochat, P., Morgan, R., Carpenter, M. (1997). Young infants' sensitivity to movement information specifying social causality. Cognitive Development, 12, 537-561
    Roth, D., & Leslie, A.M. (1998). Solving belief problems: Toward a task analysis. Cognition, 66, 1-31.
    Ruffman, T. (2000). Nonverbal theory of mind: Is it important, is it implicit, is it simulation, is it relevant to autism? In J. W. Astington (Ed.), Minds in the making: Essays in honor of David R. Olson. Oxford: Blackwell, 250-266.
    Ruffman, T., Garnham, W., & Rideout, P. (2001). Social understanding in autism: Eye gaze as a measure of core insights. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 1083-1094.
    Ruffman, T. , Garnham, W., Import , A., & Connolly, D. (2001). Does eye gaze indicate implicit knowledge of false belief? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 201-224.
    Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children's and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734-751.
    Saxe, R., Carey, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Understanding other minds: Linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of
    Psychology, 55, 87-124.
    Scholl, B., & Leslie, A. (2001). Minds, modules, and meta-analysis. Child Development, 72, 696-701.
    Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2009). Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about identity at 18 months. Child Development, 80, 1172-1196.
    Senju, A., Southgate, V., Miura, Y., Matsui, T., Hasegawa, T., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Csibra, G. (2010). Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribution in children with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 353-360.
    Senju, A., Southgate, V., Snape, C., Leonard, M., & Csibra, G. (2011). Do 18-month-olds really attribute mental states to others? Psychological Science, 22(7) 878-880.
    Slaughter, V., Peterson, C., & Mackintosh, E. (2007). Mind what mother says: narrative input and theory of mind in typical children and those on the autism spectrum. Child Development, 78(3), 839-858.
    Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Maternal mental state talk and infants' early gestural communication. Journal of Child Language, 36, 1053-1074.
    Song, H., & Baillargeon, R. (2008). Infants' reasoning about others' false perceptions. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1789-1795.
    Song, H., Onishi, K. H., Baillargeon, R., & Fisher, C. (2008). Can an agent's false belief be corrected through an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18.5-month-old infants. Cognition, 109(3), 295-315.
    Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Klinert, N. D. (1985). Maternal emotion signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 20, 195-200.
    Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by two-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18(7), 587-592.
    Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A., & Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants' knowledge of object motion andhuman action. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: Amultidisciplinary debate. New York: Oxford University Press, 44-78.
    Surian, L. and Leslie, A.M. (1999), Competence and performance in false belief understanding: A comparison of autistic and normal 3-year-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 141-155.
    Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 580-586.
    Surian, L. and Geraci, A. (2012). Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 30-44.
    Symons, D. (2004). Mental state discourse, theory of mind, and the internalization of self-other understanding. Developmental Review, 24, 159-188.
    Symons, D. K., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., Roche, J., & Doyle, E. (2005). Theory of mind and mental state discourse during book reading and story-telling tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 81-102.
    Symons, D., Fossum, K.-L., & Collins, T. (2006). A longitudinal study of belief and desire state discourse during mother-child play and later false belief understanding. Social Development, 15(4), 676-692.
    Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77, 465-481.
    Taumoepeau, M. & Ruffman, T. (2008). Stepping stones to others’ minds: Maternaltalk relates to child mental state language and emotion understanding at15, 24, and 33 months. Child Development, 79, 284-302.
    Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P., (Eds.), Joint attention : its origins and role in development. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum, 103-130.
    Träuble, B., Marinović, V., & Pauen, S. (2010). Early theory of mind competencies: do infants understand others’ beliefs? Infancy, 15, 434-444.
    Youngblade, L. M., & Dunn, J. (1995). Individual differences in young children's pretend play with mother and sibling: Links to relationships and understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs. Child Development, 66(5), 1472-1492.
    Wang, Y. H., Chang C. J., &Tsao, F.M. (2011). Two-Year-Olds’ understanding of false belief, false perception and false identity: Evidence from spontaneous-looking tasks.Paper presented at the 12th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Montreal, Canada.
    Wellman, H. M., & Bartsch, K. (1988). Young children’s reasoning about beliefs. Cognition, 30, 239-277.
    Wellman, H.M. (1990). The Child's Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs.
    The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245-275.
    Wellman, H. M. (1993). Early understanding of mind: The normal case. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 10-39.
    Wellman, H. M., & Lagattuta, K. H. (2000). Developing understandings of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience, second edition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 21-49.
    Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. & Watson, J. (2001). A meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655-684.
    Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of Theory-of-Mind Tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523-541.
    Wellman, H. M. (2010) Developing a Theory of Mind, in The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, UK.
    Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception,” Cognition, 13, 103-128
    Woodward, A.L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goalobject of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1-34. 

    無法下載圖示 本全文未授權公開
    QR CODE