簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 張書敏
Chang, Shu-Min
論文名稱: 犬群位階的線性程度與其對人類依賴程度的關連
The relationship between the linearity of dominance hierarchy and the dependence on humans in dogs
指導教授: 許鈺鸚
Hsu, Yu-Ying
口試委員: 許鈺鸚
Hsu, Yu-ying
李佩珍
Lee Shaner, Pei-Jen
顏士清
Yen, Shih-Ching
口試日期: 2024/06/27
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 生命科學系
Department of Life Science
論文出版年: 2024
畢業學年度: 112
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 81
中文關鍵詞: 遊蕩犬位階關係線性指數
英文關鍵詞: free-ranging dog, dominance hierarchy, linearity index
研究方法: 田野調查法
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202401536
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:35下載:0
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 有些群居物種會透過競爭互動建立位階關係,位階影響了牠們使用資源的順 序並降低衝突的產生。然而資源的豐富程度會影響位階的線性程度。在資源充足 的情況下,個體間無需競爭食物資源,因此位階的重要性下降。許多研究顯示犬 科動物的位階關係有明確的線性程度,優勢個體可優先使用資源,減少競爭的再 發生率。然而,有些犬群完全依賴人類供食與照顧,並與人類互動頻繁,有些則 僅部分依賴人類供食,且與人類互動少,而人類是否影響犬群位階的線性程度, 卻尚未被探討。因此,本研究目的是了解犬群對人類不同依賴程度是否影響其位 階線性程度。本研究之假說為:相較於部分依賴人類供食且與人類互動少的犬群, 完全依賴人類的犬群位階線性程度較低。本研究將犬群分類成第一類完全依賴人 類 (n = 4 群) 以及第二類部分依賴人類 (n = 2 群)。在第一類犬群中,犬隻完全依 賴人類提供充足的食物量,並與人類互動頻繁。而在第二類犬群中,犬隻僅部分 依賴人類供食,人類提供的食物量無法支撐整個犬群食用,且犬隻不常與人類接 觸,互動較少。我利用攝影機分別記錄此兩類犬群的行為,包含 (1) 服從性行為、 (2) 攻擊性行為。我使用此兩種行為計算出各犬群位階的線性指數 (Landau’s index),並比較兩類犬群群內成員間以及犬群對人的攻擊性行為表現比例是否有 差異。結果顯示,第一類中的四個犬群以兩種行為計算的位階線性指數皆無顯著 線性程度。第二類中的兩個犬群以攻擊性行為計算的位階線性指數皆無顯著線性 程度,而以服從性行為計算時,一群的位階無顯著線性程度,另一群的位階則有 顯著線性程度且優勢犬隻會優先使用食物資源。此外,相較於第二類犬群,第一 類犬群對人表現出較少的攻擊性行爲比例。本研究之結果顯示,人類會影響犬群 位階關係與犬隻行為。人類除了提供充足食物資源而影響犬群位階的線性程度外, 人類與犬隻的密切互動,也影響了犬隻對人類的行為表現。犬隻可能因爲人類掌 握食物資源,而將人類視為群體中的優勢個體。犬隻減少了對人類的攻擊性行為,並傾向與人互動,展現出更多服從性行為,此行為傾向可以增加犬隻獲得食物資 源的機會。

    Social species often establish dominance hierarchies through agonistic interactions. Dominance hierarchy influences the order in which they use resources, thereby reducing the occurrence of conflicts. However, abundant resources may affect the linearity of dominance hierarchy. When resources are sufficient, group members don’t need to compete for them, diminishing the significance of dominance hierarchy. Many studies on Canidae have shown significant linearity of dominance hierarchy. However, some groups of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are completely dependent on humans for sufficient food and interact with humans frequently, while others are partially dependent on humans for food and have little interaction with humans. Whether humans affect the linearity of dominance hierarchy in these groups has not been studied. Therefore, the aim of this study is to understand whether the different degrees of dependence of dogs on humans affect the linearity of their dominance hierarchy. I hypothesize that dog groups that are completely dependent on humans for adequate food and frequent interactions with humans will not have a clear linearity of dominance hierarchy compared to a group of dogs that are partially dependent on humans. This study classified dog groups into Full dependency (FD; n = 4) and Semi-dependency (SD; n = 2) categories. The FD category is completely dependent on humans to provide abundance food and interact frequently with humans, while the SD category is partially dependent on humans for food. The amount of food provided by humans cannot support all dogs in the SD category. Moreover, dogs don’t often come into contact with humans and have less interaction. I used camera to record the behaviors of these two dog groups, including (1) submissive behavior and (2) aggressive behavior. Each of these two behaviors were used to calculate the linearity index (Landau's index, h) of dominance hierarchy in the dog groups. I compared the differences in (1) submissive behavior and (2) aggressive behavior between the two categories. The results showed that all groups in the FD category had no significant linearity of dominance hierarchy as calculated by the two behaviors. All groups in the SD category (n = 2) had no significant linearity of dominance hierarchy based on aggressive behavior. When calculated based on submissive behavior, there was no significant linearity of dominance hierarchy in one group of the SD category, but there was significant linearity of dominance hierarchy in the other one group of the SD category, with dominant individuals having priority access to food resources. The FD category displayed less aggressive behavior towards humans than the SD category. This study demonstrates that humans are an important factor influencing the linearity of dominance hierarchy in dog groups and dog behavior. In addition to providing sufficient food resources, the close interaction between humans and dogs also affects the dogs' behavior. Dogs may consider humans as the dominant individuals in the group because humans control food resources. Dogs reduce aggressive behavior towards humans, and they tend to interact with humans and display more submissive behavior, which can increase the dog's access to food resources.

    中文摘要...................................................i Abstract......................................................................................iii 目錄....................................................v 第一章 前言 .............................................................1 第一節 群居與位階關係 ............................................1 第二節 位階關係的線性程度.............................................................3 第三節 馴養犬隻的位階關係............................................................5 第四節 人類對犬群位階關係與行為表現的影響............................7 第五節 實驗目的與假說..................................................................8 第二章 研究材料與方法 .................................................9 第一節 犬群定義與分類 ................................................... 9 第二節 實驗目標犬群與地點 .............................................. 9 第三節 實驗流程與步驟 .................................................... 14 第四節 資料分析 ...................................................... 16 第三章 結果 .................................21 第一節 犬群依賴程度是否影響犬群內位階關係之線性程度.............21 第二節 位階與進食順序以及性別是否有關聯性......................................22 第三節 犬群依賴程度是否影響群內犬隻之行為表現.....................22 第四章 討論.........................................................23 第一節 犬群位階的線性程度 .................................................24 第二節 犬群位階與進食順序以及性別之關連 ...................................27 第三節 依賴程度對犬群行為的影響 ......................................28 第四節 人類對犬隻的行為影響..............................31 結論..........................................33 參考文獻 .....................................................34 表........................................................................50 圖...............................................65

    Ang, T. Z., & Manica, A. (2010). Unavoidable limits on group size in a body size-based linear hierarchy. Behavioral Ecology, 21(4), 819–825.
    Ausband, D. E. (2018). Multiple breeding individuals within groups in a social carnivore. Journal of Mammalogy, 99(4), 836–844.
    Axelsson, E., Ratnakumar, A., Arendt, M. L., Maqbool, K., Webster, M. T., Perloski, M., Liberg, O., Arnemo, J. M., Hedhammar, A., & Lindblad-Toh, K. (2013). The genomic signature of dog domestication reveals adaptation to a starch-rich diet. Nature, 495(7441), 360–364.
    Beck, A. M. (2002). The Ecology of Stray Dogs: A Study of Free-Ranging Urban Animals. Purdue University Press.
    Beerda, B., Schilder, B. H. M., van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., de Vries, H. W., & Mol, J. A. (1999). Chronic stress in dogs subjected to social and spatial restriction. I. behavioral responses. Physiology & Behavior, 66(2), 233–242.
    Berman, M., & Dunbar, I. (1983). The social behaviour of free-ranging suburban dogs. Applied Animal Ethology, 10(1-2), 5–17.
    Bhattacharjee, D., Sarkar, R., Sau, S., & Bhadra, A. (2021). Sociability of Indian free-ranging dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) varies with human movement in urban areas. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 135(1), 89–97.
    Bhattacharjee, D., Sau, S., & Bhadra, A. (2018). Free-ranging dogs understand human intentions and adjust their behavioral responses accordingly. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 6, 232.
    Boccia, M. L., Laudenslager, M., & Reite, M. (1988). Food distribution, dominance, and aggressive behaviors in bonnet macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 16(2), 123–130.
    Boitani, L., Ciucci, P., & Ortolani, A. (2007). Behaviour and social ecology of free-ranging dogs. In The Behavioural Biology of Dogs (pp. 147–165). CABI.
    Boitani, L., & Ciucci, P. (1995). Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves. Ethology Ecology and Evolution, 7(1), 49–72.
    Boitani, L., Francisci, F., Ciucci, P., & Andreoli, G. (2016). The ecology and behavior of feral dogs: A case study from central Italy. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People: Second Edition (pp. 342–368). Cambridge University Press.
    Bonanni, R., Cafazzo, S., Fantini, C., Pontier, D., & Natoli, E. (2007). Feeding-order in an urban feral domestic cat colony: relationship to dominance rank, sex and age. Animal Behaviour, 74(5), 1369–1379.
    Bonanni, R., Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., & Natoli, E. (2010). Effect of affiliative and agonistic relationships on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Animal Behaviour, 79(5), 981–991.
    Bonanni, R., & Cafazzo, S. (2014). The social organization of a population of free-ranging dogs in a suburban area of Rome: A reassessment of the effects of domestication on dogs’ behaviour. In The Social Dog: Behavior and Cognition (pp. 65–104). Elsevier.
    Bonanni, R., Cafazzo, S., Abis, A., Barillari, E., Valsecchi, P., & Natoli, E. (2017). Age-graded dominance hierarchies and social tolerance in packs of free-ranging dogs. Behavioral Ecology, 28(4), 1004–1020.
    Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1: Attachment (2nd ed.). Basic.
    Bradshaw, J. W. S., Blackwell, E. J., & Casey, R. A. (2009). Dominance in domestic dogs-useful construct or bad habit? Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 4(3), 135–144.
    Bradshaw, J., & Rooney, N. (2016). Dog social behavior and communication. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People: Second Edition (pp. 133–159). Cambridge University Press.
    Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., Bonanni, R., & Natoli, E. (2010). Dominance in relation to age, sex, and competitive contexts in a group of free-ranging domestic dogs. Behavioral Ecology, 21(3), 443–455.
    Cafazzo, S., Lazzaroni, M., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2016). Dominance relationships in a family pack of captive arctic wolves (Canis lupus arctos): The influence of competition for food, age and sex. PeerJ, 4, e2707.
    Cangialosi, K. R. (1990). Social spider defense against kleptoparasitism. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 27, 49–54.
    Cassidy, K. A., MacNulty, D. R., Stahler, D. R., Smith, D. W., & Mech, L. D. (2015). Group composition effects on aggressive interpack interactions of gray wolves in Yellowstone National Park. Behavioral Ecology, 26(5), 1352–1360.
    Cavalli, C., Dzik, V., Carballo, F., & Bentosela, M. (2016). Post-conflict affiliative behaviors towards humans in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 29(1), 1–13.
    Cerda, X., Retana, J., & Cros, S. (1997). Thermal disruption of transitive hierarchies in Mediterranean ant communities. Journal of Animal Ecology, 66(3), 363–374.
    Cerda, X., Retana, J., & Cros Cerda, S. (1998). Prey size reverses the outcome of interference interactions of scavenger ants. Oikos, 82, 99–110.
    Cimarelli, G., Virányi, Z., Turcsán, B., Rónai, Z., Sasvári-Székely, M., & Bánlaki, Z. (2017). Social behavior of pet dogs is associated with peripheral OXTR methylation. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 549.
    Clutton-Brock, T. H., Hodge, S. J., Spong, G., Russell, A. F., Jordan, N. R., Bennett, N. C., Sharpe, L. L., & Manser, M. B. (2006). Intrasexual competition and sexual selection in cooperative mammals. Nature, 444(7122), 1065–1068.
    Conradt, L., & Roper, T. J. (2003). Group decision-making in animals. Nature, 421(6919), 155–158.
    Coppinger, R., & Feinstein, M. (2020). How dogs work. University of Chicago Press.
    Courchamp, F., & Macdonald, D. W. (2001). Crucial importance of pack size in the African wild dog Lycaon pictus. Animal Conservation, 4(2), 169–174.
    Cowan, D. P. (1987). Group living in the European rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus): mutual benefit or resource localization? The Journal of Animal Ecology, 56, 779–795.
    Creel, S., & Creel, N. M. (1995). Communal hunting and pack size in African wild dogs, Lycaon pictus. Animal Behaviour, 50(5), 1325–1339.
    Curveira‐Santos, G., Gigliotti, L., Silva, A. P., Sutherland, C., Foord, S., Santos‐Reis, M., & Swanepoel, L. H. (2022). Broad aggressive interactions among African carnivores suggest intraguild killing is driven by more than competition. Ecology, 103(2), e03600.
    Daniels, T. J. (1983a). The social organization of free-ranging urban dogs. I. Non-estrous social behavior. Applied Animal Ethology, 10(4), 341–363.
    Daniels, T. J. (1983b). The social organization of free-ranging urban dogs II. Estrous groups and the mating system. Applied Animal Ethology, 10(4), 365–373.
    Daniels, T. J., & Bekoff, M. (1989a). Population and social biology of free-ranging dogs, Canis familiaris. Journal of Mammalogy, 70(4), 754–762.
    Daniels, T. J., & Bekoff, M. (1989b). Spatial and temporal resource use by feral and abandoned dogs. Ethology, 81(4), 300–312.
    Dale, R., Range, F., Stott, L., Kotrschal, K., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2017). The influence of social relationship on food tolerance in wolves and dogs. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 71, 1–14.
    Davis, J. M. (1975). Socially induced flight reactions in pigeons. Animal Behaviour, 23, 597–601.
    Davis, R. S., Yarnell, R. W., Gentle, L. K., Uzal, A., Mgoola, W. O., & Stone, E. L. (2021). Prey availability and intraguild competition regulate the spatiotemporal dynamics of a modified large carnivore guild. Ecology and Evolution, 11(12), 7890–7904.
    Day, R. L., MacDonald, T., Brown, C., Laland, K. N., & Reader, S. M. (2001). Interactions between shoal size and conformity in guppy social foraging. Animal Behaviour, 62(5), 917–925.
    De Villiers, M. S., Richardson, P. R., & Van Jaarsveld, A. S. (2003). Patterns of coalition formation and spatial association in a social carnivore, the African wild dog (Lycaon pictus). Journal of Zoology, 260(4), 377–389.
    de Vries, H. (1995). An improved test of linearity in dominance hierarchies containing unknown or tied relationships. Animal Behaviour, 50(5), 1375–1389.
    de Vries, H. (1998). Finding a dominance order most consistent with a linear hierarchy: a new procedure and review. Animal Behaviour, 55(4), 827–843.
    de Waal, F. B. M. (1986). The integration of dominance and social bonding in primates. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 61(4), 459–479.
    dos Santos, C. L. A., le Pendu, Y., Giné, G. A. F., Dickman, C. R., Newsome, T. M., & Cassano, C. R. (2018). Human behaviors determine the direct and indirect impacts of free-ranging dogs on wildlife. Journal of Mammalogy, 99(5), 1261–1269.
    Duncan, P., & Vigne, N. (1979). The effect of group size in horses on the rate of attacks by blood-sucking flies. Animal Behaviour, 27, 623–625.
    Duranton, C., & Gaunet, F. (2018). Behavioral synchronization and affiliation: Dogs exhibit human-like skills. Learning and Behavior, 46(4), 364–373.
    Feddersen-Petersen, D. (1991). The ontogeny of social play and agonistic behaviour in selected canid species. Bonner zoologische Beiträge, 42, 97–114.
    Fels, M., Hartung, J., & Hoy, S. (2014). Social hierarchy formation in piglets mixed in different group compositions after weaning. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 152, 17–22.
    Foster, W. A., & Treherne, J. E. (1981). Evidence for the dilution effect in the selfish herd from fish predation on a marine insect. Nature, 293(5832), 466–467.
    Fox, M. W., Beck, A. M., & Blackman, E. (1975). Behavior and ecology of a small group of urban dogs (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Ethology, 1(2), 119–137.
    Frank, H., & Frank, M. G. (1982). On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior. Applied Animal Ethology, 8(6), 507–525.
    Galimberti, F., Fabiani, A., & Boitani, L. (2003). Socio-spatial levels in linearity analysis of dominance hierarchies: A case study on elephant seals. Journal of Ethology, 21(2), 131–136.
    Gittleman, J. L. (1989). Carnivore group living: Comparative trends. In Carnivore Behavior, Ecology, and Evolution (pp. 183–207). Springer.
    Guo, S. T., He, S. J., Zhang, H., Bai, R. F., Zhang, S. M., Hou, R., Grueter, C. C., Chapman, C. A., Dunn, D. W., & Li, B. G. (2020). Male social rank and food competition in a primate multi-level society. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 173(4), 630–642.
    Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439–444.
    Hart, B. L., & Eckstein, R. A. (1997). The role of gonadal hormones in the occurrence of objectionable behaviours in dogs and cats. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 52(3-4), 331–344.
    Harrington, F. H., David Mech, L., & Fritts, S. H. (1983). Pack size and wolf pup survival: Their relationship under varying ecological conditions. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 13(1), 19–26.
    Hayes, L. D., & Solomon, N. G. (2004). Costs and benefits of communal rearing to female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56, 585–593.
    Howell, T., King, T., & Bennett, P. (2015). Puppy parties and beyond: the role of early age socialization practices on adult dog behavior. Veterinary Medicine: Research and Reports, 6, 143–153.
    Hrdy, S. B. (1976). Care and exploitation of nonhuman primate infants by conspecifics other than the mother. In Advances in the Study of Behavior (pp. 101–158). Academic Press.
    Jackson, C. R., Groom, R. J., Jordan, N. R., & McNutt, J. W. (2017). The effect of relatedness and pack size on territory overlap in African wild dogs. Movement Ecology, 5, 1–10.
    Kaminski, J., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2014). The social dog: behavior and cognition. Elsevier.
    Kittle, A. M., Anderson, M., Avgar, T., Baker, J. A., Brown, G. S., Hagens, J., Iwachewski, E., Moffatt, S., Mosser, A., Patterson, B. R., Reid, D. E. B., Rodgers, A. R., Shuter, J., Street, G. M., Thompson, I. D., vander Vennen, L. M., & Fryxell, J. M. (2015). Wolves adapt territory size, not pack size to local habitat quality. Journal of Animal Ecology, 84(5), 1177–1186.
    King, A. J., Douglas, C. M. S., Huchard, E., Isaac, N. J. B., & Cowlishaw, G. (2008). Dominance and Affiliation Mediate Despotism in a Social Primate. Current Biology, 18(23), 1833–1838.
    Klass, K., & Cords, M. (2011). Effect of unknown relationships on linearity, steepness and rank ordering of dominance hierarchies: Simulation studies based on data from wild monkeys. Behavioural Processes, 88(3), 168–176.
    König, B. (1997). Cooperative care of young in mammals. Naturwissenschaften, 84(3), 95–104.
    Krause, J., & Ruxton, G. (2002). Living in groups. Oxford University Press.
    Kriese, M., Kuźniewska, E., Gugołek, A., & Strychalski, J. (2022). Reasons for and behavioral consequences of male dog castration—A questionnaire study in Poland. Animals, 12(15), 1883.
    Landau, H. G. (1951). On dominance relations and the structure of animal societies: I. Effect of inherent characteristics. The bulletin of mathematical biophysics, 13, 1–19.
    Landsberg, G., Hunthausen, W., & Ackerman, L. (2011). Behavior problems of the dog and cat. Elsevier Health Sciences.
    LeBrun, E. G. (2005). Who is the top dog in ant communities? Resources, parasitoids, and multiple competitive hierarchies. Oecologia, 142(4), 643–652.
    Lehmann, J., Korstjens, A. H., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates. Animal Behaviour, 74(6), 1617–1629.
    Leiva, D., Solanas, A., & Salafranca, L. (2008). Testing reciprocity in social interactions: A comparison between the directional consistency and skew-symmetry statistics. Behavior Research Methods, 40(2), 626–634.
    Lindstrom, E. (1986). Territory inheritance and the evolution of group-living in carnivores. Animal Behaviour, 34(6), 1825–1835.
    Lin, Y. H., Chan, S. F., Rubenstein, D. R., Liu, M., & Shen, S. F. (2019). Resolving the paradox of environmental quality and sociality: The ecological causes and consequences of cooperative breeding in two lineages of birds. The American Naturalist, 194(2), 207–216.
    Lord, K., Feinstein, M., Smith, B., & Coppinger, R. (2013). Variation in reproductive traits of members of the genus Canis with special attention to the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Behavioural Processes, 92, 131–142.
    Macdonald, D. W., & Carr, G. M. (2016). Variation in dog society: Between resource dispersion and social flux. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People: Second Edition (pp. 319–341). Cambridge University Press.
    Majolo, B., & Huang, P. (2018). Group Living. In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior (pp. 1–12). Springer International Publishing.
    Mariti, C., Carlone, B., Ricci, E., Sighieri, C., & Gazzano, A. (2014). Intraspecific attachment in adult domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): Preliminary results. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 152, 64–72.
    Marshall-Pescini, S., Cafazzo, S., Virányi, Z., & Range, F. (2017). Integrating social ecology in explanations of wolf-dog behavioral differences. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 16, 80–86.
    Mascaro, O., Goupil, N., Pantecouteau, H., Depierreux, A., Van der Henst, J. B., & Claidière, N. (2023). Human and animal dominance hierarchies show a pyramidal structure guiding adult and infant social inferences. Nature Human Behaviour, 7(8), 1294–1306.
    Mech, L. D. (1999). Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 77(8), 1196–1203.
    Meek, P. D. (1999). The movement, roaming behaviour and home range of free-roaming domestic dogs, Canis lupus familiaris, in coastal New South Wales. Wildlife Research, 26(6), 847–855.
    Michel, E. S., Demarais, S., Strickland, B. K., Belant, J. L., & Millspaugh, J. J. (2016). Quantifying dominance of adult female white-tailed deer in the presence of abundant food. Behaviour, 153(1), 49–67.
    Nagasawa, M., Mitsui, S., En, S., Ohtani, N., Ohta, M., Sakuma, Y., Onaka, T., Mogi, K., & Kikusui, T. (2015). Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science, 348(6232), 333–336.
    Natoli, E., Baggio, A., & Pontier, D. (2001). Male and female agonistic and affiliative relationships in a social group of farm cats (Felis catus L.). Behavioural Processes, 53(1-2), 137–143.
    Neilson, J. C., Eckstein, R. A., & Hart, B. L. (1997). Effects of castration on problem behaviors in male dogs with reference to age and duration of behavior. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 211(2), 180–182.
    Ortolani, A., Vernooij, H., & Coppinger, R. (2009). Ethiopian village dogs: Behavioural responses to a stranger’s approach. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 119(3-4), 210–218.
    Pal, S. K., Ghosh, B., & Roy, S. (1998). Agonistic behaviour of free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to season, sex and age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 59(4), 331–348.
    Pal, S. K., Ghosh, B., & Roy, S. (1999). Inter- and intra-sexual behaviour of free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 66(2-3), 267–278.
    Pal, S. K. (2015). Factors influencing intergroup agonistic behaviour in free-ranging domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Acta Ethologica, 18(2), 209–220.
    Palagi, E., & Norscia, I. (2015). The socio-matrix reloaded: from hierarchy to dominance profile in wild lemurs. PeerJ, 3, e729.
    Papageorgiou, D., & Farine, D. R. (2020). Group size and composition influence collective movement in a highly social terrestrial bird. ELife, 9, e59902.
    Perri, A. (2016). A wolf in dog’s clothing: Initial dog domestication and Pleistocene wolf variation. Journal of Archaeological Science, 68, 1–4.
    Perry, B. D. (1993). Dog ecology in eastern and southern Africa: implications for rabies control. Onderstepoort Journal of Veterinary Research, 60, 429–436.
    Pilot, M., Greco, C., vonHoldt, B. M., Jędrzejewska, B., Randi, E., Jędrzejewski, W., Sidorovich, V. E., Ostrander, E. A., & Wayne, R. K. (2014). Genome-wide signatures of population bottlenecks and diversifying selection in European wolves. Heredity, 112(4), 428–442.
    Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, A., Timár-Geng, K., & Csányi, V. (2004). Verbal attention getting as a key factor in social learning between dog (Canis familiaris) and human. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118(4), 375–383.
    Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Vida, V., & Csányi, V. (2005). The pet dogs ability for learning from a human demonstrator in a detour task is independent from the breed and age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90(3-4), 309–323.
    Range, F., Ritter, C., & Virányi, Z. (2015). Testing the myth: Tolerant dogs and aggressive wolves. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1807), 20150220.
    Range, F., Marshall-Pescini, S., Kratz, C., & Virányi, Z. (2019). Wolves lead and dogs follow, but they both cooperate with humans. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 3796.
    Reisner, I. R. (1997). Assessment, management, and prognosis of canine dominance-related aggression. Veterinary clinics of North America: Small animal practice, 27(3), 479–495.
    Ross, H. E., & Young, L. J. (2009). Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social cognition and affiliative behavior. Frontiers in neuroendocrinology, 30(4), 534–547.
    Ruiz-Izaguirre, E., Eilers, K. H. A. M., Bokkers, E. A. M., Ortolani, A., Ortega-Pacheco, A., & de Boer, I. J. M. (2014). Human-dog interactions and behavioural responses of village dogs in coastal villages in Michoacán, Mexico. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 154, 57–65.
    Saavedra-Aracena, L., Grimm-Seyfarth, A., & Schüttler, E. (2021). Do dog-human bonds influence movements of free-ranging dogs in wilderness? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 241, 105358.
    Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Social status and health in humans and other animals. Annual Review Anthropology, 33, 393–418.
    Schenkel, R. (1967). Submission: its features and function in the wolf and dog. American Zoologist, 7(2), 319–329.
    Schilder, M. B. H., Vinke, C. M., & van der Borg, J. A. M. (2014). Dominance in domestic dogs revisited: Useful habit and useful construct? Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 9(4), 184–191.
    sen Majumder, S., Chatterjee, A., & Bhadra, A. (2014). A dog’s day with humans-time activity budget of free-ranging dogs in India. Current Science, 106, 874–878.
    Sheppard, J. K., Walenski, M., Wallace, M. P., Velazco, J. J. V., Porras, C., & Swaisgood, R. R. (2013). Hierarchical dominance structure in reintroduced California condors: Correlates, consequences, and dynamics. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 67(8), 1227–1238.
    Shizuka, D., & McDonald, D. B. (2015). The network motif architecture of dominance hierarchies. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 12(105), 20150080.
    Silk, M. J., Cant, M. A., Cafazzo, S., Natoli, E., & McDonald, R. A. (2019). Elevated aggression is associated with uncertainty in a network of dog dominance interactions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286, 20190536.
    Siracusa, C. (2021). Aggression-dogs. In Small animal veterinary psychiatry (pp. 191–206). CABI.
    Sterck, E. H., Watts, D. P., & Van Schaik, C. P. (1997). The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behavioral ecology and sociobiology, 41, 291–309.
    Stewart, R. B. (1983). Sibling attachment relationships: child-infant interactions in the strange situation. Developmental Psychology, 19(2), 192–199.
    Takeshita, R. S. C., Huffman, M. A., Kinoshita, K., & Bercovitch, F. B. (2017). Effect of castration on social behavior and hormones in male Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Physiology and Behavior, 181, 43–50.
    Taj, M. K., Hassani, I. T., & Mustafa, M. Z. (2015). Stray dogs life in Quetta city of Balochistan. Advanced Online Publication Articles for HFSP Journal, 9, 36–40.
    Tibbetts, E. A., Pardo-Sanchez, J., & Weise, C. (2022). The establishment and maintenance of dominance hierarchies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1845), 20200450.
    Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V. (1997). Dog-human relationship affects problem solving behavior in the dog. Anthrozoös, 10(4), 214–224.
    Trisko, R. K. (2011). Dominance, egalitarianism and friendship at a dog daycare facility (Order No. 3459077). Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses A&I; ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (872984539).
    Trisko, R. K., & Smuts, B. B. (2015). Dominance relationships in a group of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Behaviour, 152(5), 677–704.
    Tsai, Y. J. J., Barrows, E. M., & Weiss, M. R. (2014). Why do larger and older males win contests in the parasitoid wasp Nasonia vitripennis? Animal behaviour, 91, 151–159.
    Tuber, D. S., Hennessy, M. B., Sanders, S., & Miller, J. A. (1996). Behavioral and glucocorticoid responses of adult domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to companionship and social separation. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110(1), 103–108.
    Tuyttens, F. A. M., Stapley, N., Stewart, P. D., & Macdonald, D. W. (2001). Vigilance in badgers Meles meles: the effects of group size and human persecution. Acta Theriologica, 46(1), 79–86.
    Urfer, S. R., & Kaeberlein, M. (2019). Desexing dogs: A review of the current literature. Animals, 9(12), 1086.
    van der Borg, J. A. M., Schilder, M. B. H., Vinke, C. M., de Vries, H., & Petit, O. (2015). Dominance in domestic dogs: A quantitative analysis of its behavioural measures. PLoS ONE, 10(8), e0133978.
    van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., & Wensing, J. A. (1987). Dominance and its behavioral measures in a captive wolf pack. In Man and Wolf: Advances, Issues, and Problems in Captive Wolf Research (pp. 219–252). Springer Science & Business Media.
    van Kerkhove, W. (2004). A fresh look at the wolf-pack theory of companion-animal dog social behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 7(4), 279–285.
    Vanak, A. T., Thaker, M., & Gompper, M. E. (2009). Experimental examination of behavioural interactions between free-ranging wild and domestic canids. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64(2), 279–287.
    Vanak, A. T., & Gompper, M. E. (2009). Dogs Canis familiaris as carnivores: their role and function in intraguild competition. Mammal review, 39(4), 265–283.
    Vervaecke, H., & Stevens, J. (2019). Dominance hierarchy. In Encyclopedia of ecology (pp. 192–196). Elsevier.
    Virányi, Z., & Range, F. (2014). On the way to a better understanding of dog domestication: aggression and cooperativeness in dogs and wolves. In The Social Dog (pp. 35–62). Academic Press.
    Waeber, P. O., & Hemelrijk, C. K. (2003). Female dominance and social structure in Alaotran gentle lemurs. Behaviour, 140(10), 1235–1246.
    Watts, D. P. (1985). Relations between group size and composition and feeding competition in mountain gorilla groups. Animal Behaviour, 33(1), 72–85.
    Wang, M. Y., Ruckstuhl, K. E., Xu, W. X., Blank, D., & Yang, W. K. (2016). Human activity dampens the benefits of group size on vigilance in Khulan (Equus hemionus) in Western China. PLoS One, 11(1), e0146725.
    West, P. M., & Packer, C. (2002). Sexual selection, temperature, and the lion’s man
    e. Science, 297(5585), 1339–1343.
    Wittig, R. M., & Boesch, C. (2002). Food competition and linear dominance hierarchy among female chimpanzees of the Ta ̈ı National Park. International Journal of Primatology, 24(4), 847–867.
    Wittemyer, G., & Getz, W. M. (2007). Hierarchical dominance structure and social organization in African elephants, Loxodonta africana. Animal Behaviour, 73(4), 671–681.
    Willen, R. M., Schiml, P. A., & Hennessy, M. B. (2019). Enrichment centered on human interaction moderates fear-induced aggression and increases positive expectancy in fearful shelter dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 217, 57–62.
    Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology, the New Synthesis. Harvard University Press.
    World Health Organization. (1988). Report of WHO Consultation on Dog Ecology Studies Related to Rabies Control, Geneva, 22–25 February 1988 (No. WHO/Rab. Res./88.25. Unpublished). World Health Organization.
    Wynne, C. D. L. (2021a). Dogs’ (Canis lupus familiaris) behavioral adaptations to a human-dominated niche: A review and novel hypothesis. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 53, 972–162.
    Wynne, C. D. L. (2021b). The Indispensable Dog. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 656529.
    林健洲、林曜松、 周蓮香 (2000)。陽明山國家公園龍鳳谷遊憩區流浪犬之社會結構與行為互動。內政部營建署陽明山國家公園管理處。

    下載圖示
    QR CODE