簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 陳仙霈
Chen, Hsien-Pei
論文名稱: 遮蔽物使用傾向與打鬥能力對北美紅樹林鱂魚之擁有者優勢的影響
The influences of sheltering tendency and fighting ability on the ownership advantage in Kryptolebias marmoratus
指導教授: 許鈺鸚
Hsu, Yu-ying
口試委員: 許鈺鸚
Hsu, Yu-ying
李壽先
Li, Shou-Hsien
林惠真
Lin, Hui-Chen
口試日期: 2024/07/11
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 生命科學系
Department of Life Science
論文出版年: 2024
畢業學年度: 112
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 44
中文關鍵詞: 打鬥能力資源擁有狀態擁有者優勢遮蔽物使用傾向北美紅樹林鱂魚
英文關鍵詞: fighting ability, resource ownership, ownership advantage, shelter tendency, Kryptolebias marmoratus
研究方法: 實驗設計法比較研究觀察研究
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202401869
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:46下載:0
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 在競爭資源的行為中,資源價值與動物的打鬥能力會是影響其決定是否投入打鬥的重要依據,資源價值可使個體評估打鬥獲得的好處,而打鬥能力則會影響個體評估打鬥中需付出的代價。資源擁有狀態的差異也可提供擁有資源的個體競爭上的優勢,因為資源擁有者通常攻擊性較強,較容易在打鬥中獲勝,此稱為擁有者優勢(ownership advantage)。擁有者優勢可能受到打鬥中其他因子的影響,如打鬥能力和/或打鬥經驗,導致不同打鬥能力的擁有者有不同的表現。在使用遮蔽物作為資源的物種中,不同個體可能因對遮蔽物評估的價值不同而影響使用遮蔽物的傾向,對遮蔽物的偏好會影響競爭的強度。本研究之目的為了解打鬥能力與遮蔽物使用程度如何影響擁有者優勢的展現。本研究使用北美紅樹林鱂魚(Kryptolebias marmoratus)作為實驗物種,提供遮蔽物作為資源,設計了四個實驗處理組:A. 雙方皆打鬥能力較好,僅目標個體(隨機決定)有遮蔽物、B. 雙方皆打鬥能力較差,僅目標個體(隨機決定)有遮蔽物、C. 目標個體打鬥能力較差且擁有遮蔽物,其對手打鬥能力較好但無遮蔽物、D. 目標個體打鬥能力較差,其對手打鬥能力較好,雙方皆無遮蔽物。我以雙方打鬥能力相同的A、B處理組檢測配對雙方的打鬥能力與遮蔽物使用程度對擁有者優勢與配對雙方打鬥意願之影響,以雙方打鬥能力不同的C、D處理組檢測打鬥能力較差的個體有無遮蔽物與遮蔽物使用程度對其獲勝率與雙方打鬥意願之影響。實驗結果發現雙方打鬥能力相同(A、B處理組)時,配對雙方的打鬥能力對擁有者優勢沒有影響,在雙方打鬥能力皆較差(B處理組)時有偵測到顯著的擁有者優勢,而雙方打鬥能力皆較好(A處理組)時其擁有者優勢則是接近顯著。而雙方打鬥能力不同(C、D處理組)時,打鬥能力較差的擁有者因為擁有遮蔽物,獲勝率得以提升,但仍低於打鬥能力較好的對手,顯示擁有者優勢無法彌補打鬥能力的劣勢。個體使用遮蔽物的程度只在雙方打鬥能力相同時有影響:入侵者越傾向使用遮蔽物,其打鬥意願越高;而擁有者越傾向使用遮蔽物,則越能展現擁有者優勢。

    When animals compete for resources, the resource value and the animal's fighting ability will be important factors in deciding whether to engage in fighting. The resource value lets individuals know the benefit that can be gained from fighting, while an individual's fighting ability will affect the assessment of the cost of fighting. Differences in resource ownership status can also provide resource owners with a competitive advantage because resource owners are usually more aggressive and are easier to win in fights. This is called the owner advantage. The owner's advantage may be affected by other factors in fighting, such as fighting ability and/or fighting experience, causing owners with different fighting abilities to perform differently. In species that use shelter as a resource, individuals may assess the value of shelter differently, which may affect the tendency to use shelter and continuously affect the intensity of the contest. The aim of this study is to understand how fighting ability and sheltering tendency influence owner advantage. The experiment species for this study is Kryptolebias marmoratus. I designed 4 treatments to answer the questions: A. Both contestants have better fighting ability; only the focal individual (randomly assigned) has a shelter; B. Both contestants have worse fighting ability; only the focal individual (randomly assigned) has a shelter; C. The focal individual has worse fighting ability and shelter, and its opponent has better fighting ability but has no shelter; D. Both contestants have no resources. The focal individual has worse fighting ability, and its opponent has better fighting ability. I used treatment A and B to test how fighting ability and sheltering tendency influence owner advantage and the willingness of both contestants to fight. The treatment C and D were used to detect whether the individual has a shelter and sheltering tendency affects the winning probability and the willingness of both contestants to fight. The results show that in treatment A and B, the fighting ability doesn't impact the owner advantage. When both individuals have worse fighting abilities (treatment B), a significant owner advantage is detected; and when both individuals have better fighting abilities (treatment A), the owner advantage is close to significant. In treatment C and D, the owner with worse fighting ability has an improved winning probability because it has a shelter. But it’s still lower than the opponent with better fighting ability. This indicates the owner advantage can’t offset the disadvantage of fighting ability. The sheltering tendency of an individual only has an influence when the fighting ability of both individuals is the same: the more the intruder tends to use shelter, the higher its willingness to fight; and the more the owner tends to use shelter, the more owner advantage can be detected.

    摘要 i Abstract iii 目錄 v 第一章 前言 1 第一節 影響動物打鬥結果的因子 1 第二節 擁有者優勢 2 第三節 影響擁有者優勢的因子 4 第四節 遮蔽物為重要資源與使用遮蔽物程度對打鬥的影響 5 第五節 研究目的 7 第二章 材料與方法 9 第一節 研究物種 9 第二節 實驗設計 10 第三節 實驗流程 10 第四節 資料分析 14 第三章 結果 16 第一節 遮蔽物擁有者之優勢是否存在 16 第二節 打鬥能力與遮蔽物使用程度如何影響擁有者優勢 17 第四章 討論 20 參考文獻 26 表 33 圖 39

    Alcock, J., & Bailey, W. (1997). Success in territorial defence by male tarantula hawk wasps Hemipepsis ustulata: The role of residency. Ecological Entomology, 22(4), 377-383.
    Arnott, G., & Elwood, R. W. (2007). Fighting for shells: How private information about resource value changes hermit crab pre-fight displays and escalated fight behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1628), 3011–3017.
    Arnott, G., & Elwood, R. W. (2008). Information gathering and decision making about resource value in animal contests. Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 529–542.
    Beaugrand, J. P., Payette, D., & Goulet, C. (1996). Conflict outcome in male green swordtail fish dyads (Xiphophorus helleri): Interaction of body size, prior dominance/subordination experience, and prior residency. Behaviour, 133(3–4), 303–319.
    Brown, C., & Braithwaite, V. A. (2004). Size matters: a test of boldness in eight populations of the poeciliid Brachyraphis episcopi. Animal Behaviour, 68(6), 1325-1329.
    Buena, L. J., & Walker, S. E. (2008). Information asymmetry and aggressive behaviour in male house crickets, Acheta domesticus. Animal Behaviour, 75(1), 199-204.
    Chang, C., Li, C.-Y., Earley, R. L., & Hsu, Y. (2012). Aggression and related behavioral traits: The impact of winning and losing and the role of hormones. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 52(6), 801–813.
    Davies, N. B. (1978). Territorial defence in the speckled wood butterfly (Pararge aegeria): The resident always wins. Animal Behaviour, 26, 138–147.
    Driscoll, J., Kola, M., & Mathews, L. (2020). Perception of alarm cues influences the outcome of shelter competition in crayfish20. Ethology, 126(5), 584-592.
    Dowling, L. M., & Godin, J.-G. J. (2002). Refuge use in a killifish: Influence of body size and nutritional state. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 80(4), 782–788.
    Enquist, M., & Leimar, O. (1987). Evolution of fighting behaviour: The effect of variation in resource value. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 127(2), 187–205.
    Enquist, M., & Leimar, O. (1990). The evolution of fatal fighting. Animal Behaviour, 39(1), 1–9.
    Fayed, S. A., Jennions, M. D., & Backwell, P. R. Y. (2008). What factors contribute to an ownership advantage? Biology Letters, 4(2), 143–145.
    Fero, K. C., & Moore, P. A. (2014). Shelter availability influences social behavior and habitat choice in crayfish, Orconectes virilis. Behaviour, 151(1), 103–123.
    Figler, M. H., Blank, G. S., & Peeke, H. V. (1997). Maternal aggression and post‐hatch care in red swamp crayfish, Procambarus clarkii (Girard): The influences of presence of offspring, fostering, and maternal molting. Marine & Freshwater Behaviour & Phy, 30(3), 173-194.
    Frost, A. J., Winrow-Giffen, A., Ashley, P. J., & Sneddon, L. U. (2007). Plasticity in animal personality traits: Does prior experience alter the degree of boldness?. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1608), 333-339.
    Gherardi, F. (2006). Fighting behavior in hermit crabs: The combined effect of resource-holding potential and resource value in Pagurus longicarpus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 59, 500-510.
    Gosling LM, McKay HV (1990) Competitor assessment by scent-matching: An experimental test. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26:415-420
    Gruber, C., Tulonen, J., Kortet, R., & Hirvonen, H. (2016). Resource availability and predation risk influence contest behavior and dominance hierarchies in crayfish. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 70(8), 1305–1317.
    Guerra-Grenier, E., Abram, P. K., & Brodeur, J. (2020). Asymmetries affecting aggressive contests between solitary parasitoids: The effect of host species. Behavioral Ecology, 31(6), 1391-1400.
    Hsu, Y., & Wolf, L. L. (1999). The winner and loser effect: Integrating multiple experiences. Animal Behaviour, 57(4), 903–910.
    Hsu, Y., Earley, R. L., & Wolf, L. L. (2006). Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: Mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biological Reviews, 81(01), 33.
    Huntingford, F. A., & Turner, A. K. (1987). Animal conflict. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.
    Huang, S. P., Yang, S. Y., & Hsu, Y. (2011). Persistence of winner and loser effects depends on the behaviour measured. Ethology, 117(2), 171-180.
    Huang, W., & Hsu, Y. (2015). Asymmetrical resource ownership increases owners’ and reduces non‐owners’ motivation to fight in the mangrove rivulus, Kryptolebias marmoratus. Ethology, 121(9), 915–921.
    Johnsson, J. I., Carlsson, M., & Sundström, L. F. (2000). Habitat preference increases territorial defence in brown trout (Salmo trutta). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 48(5), 373–377.
    Kasumovic, M. M., Mason, A. C., Andrade, M. C. B., & Elias, D. O. (2011). The relative importance of RHP and resource quality in contests with ownership asymmetries. Behavioral Ecology, 22(1), 39–45.
    Kemp, D. J., & Wiklund, C. (2004). Residency effects in animal contests. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 271(1549), 1707-1711.
    Kokko, H., López-Sepulcre, A., & Morrell, L. J. (2006). From hawks and doves to self-consistent games of territorial behavior. The American Naturalist, 167(6), 901-912.
    Lan, Y.-T., & Hsu, Y. (2011). Prior contest experience exerts a long-term influence on subsequent winner and loser effects. Frontiers in Zoology, 8(1), 28.
    Leimar, O., & Enquist, M. (1984). Effects of asymmetries in owner-intruder conflicts. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 111(3), 475–491.
    Lindström, K., & Pampoulie, C. (2005). Effects of resource holding potential and resource value on tenure at nest sites in sand gobies. Behavioral Ecology, 16(1), 70-74.
    Lubinski, B. A., Davis, W. P., Taylor, D. S., & Turner, B. J. (1995). Outcrossing in a natural population of a self-fertilizing hermaphroditic fish. Journal of Heredity, 86(6), 469–473.
    Mackiewicz, M., Tatarenkov, A., Taylor, D. S., Turner, B. J., & Avise, J. C. (2006a). Extensive outcrossing and androdioecy in a vertebrate species that otherwise reproduces as a self-fertilizing hermaphrodite. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(26), 9924–9928.
    Mackiewicz, M., Tatarenkov, A., Turner, B. J., & Avise, J. C. (2006b). A mixed-mating strategy in a hermaphroditic vertebrate. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1600), 2449–2452.
    Martin, A. L., & Moore, P. A. (2008). The influence of dominance on shelter preference and eviction rates in the crayfish, Orconectes rusticus. Ethology, 114(4), 351–360.
    Mathews, L. (2021). Outcomes of agonistic interactions alter sheltering behavior in crayfish. Behavioural Processes, 184, 104337.
    Molloy, P. P., Nyboer, E. A., & Côté, I. M. (2011). Male-male competition in a mixed-mating fish: Male-male competition in a mixed-mating fish. Ethology, 117(7), 586–596.
    Neat, F. C., Taylor, A. C., & Huntingford, F. A. (1998). Proximate costs of fighting in male cichlid fish: The role of injuries and energy metabolism. Animal Behaviour, 55(4), 875-882.
    Nosil, P. (2002). Food fights in house crickets, Acheta domesticus, and the effects of body size and hunger level. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 80(3), 409-417.
    O'Connor, C. M., Reddon, A. R., Ligocki, I. Y., Hellmann, J. K., Garvy, K. A., Marsh-Rollo, S. E., ... & Balshine, S. (2015). Motivation but not body size influences territorial contest dynamics in a wild cichlid fish. Animal Behaviour, 107, 19-29.
    Olsson, K., & Nyström, P. (2009). Non‐interactive effects of habitat complexity and adult crayfish on survival and growth of juvenile crayfish (Pacifastacus leniusculus). Freshwater Biology, 54(1), 35–46.
    Parker, G. A. (1974). Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 47(1), 223–243.
    Peixoto, P. E. C., & Benson, W. W. (2008). Body mass and not wing length predicts territorial success in a tropical satyrine butterfly. Ethology, 114(11), 1069-1077.
    Sacchi, R., Pupin, F., Gentilli, A., Rubolini, D., Scali, S., Fasola, M., & Galeotti, P. (2009). Male–male combats in a polymorphic lizard: Residency and size, but not color, affect fighting rules and contest outcome. Aggressive Behavior: Official Journal of the International Society for Research on Aggression, 35(3), 274-283.
    Sih, A. (1997). To hide or not to hide? Refuge use in a fluctuating environment. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 12(10), 375-376.
    Smith, J. M. (1974). The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts. Journal of theoretical biology, 47(1), 209-221.
    Smith, J. M., & Parker, G. A. (1976). The logic of asymmetric contests. Animal Behaviour, 24(1), 159–175.
    Sneddon, L. U. (2003). The bold and the shy: Individual differences in rainbow trout. Journal of Fish Biology, 62(4), 971-975.
    Takahashi, K., Yamaguchi, E., Fujiyama, N., & Nagayama, T. (2019). The effects of quality of shelters and prior residence on Marmorkrebs (marbled crayfish). Journal of Experimental Biology, jeb.197301.
    Tanner, C. J., Salalι, G. D., & Jackson, A. L. (2011). Feeding and non-feeding aggression can be induced in invasive shore crabs by altering food distribution. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65, 249-256.
    Taylor, D. S. (1990). Adaptive specializations of the cyprinodont fish Rivulus marmoratus. Florida Scientist, 53(3), 239–248.
    Taylor, D. S. (2012). Twenty-four years in the mud: What have we learned about the natural history and ecology of the mangrove rivulus, Kryptolebias marmoratus? Integrative and Comparative Biology, 52(6), 724–736.
    Tina, F. W., Jaroensutasinee, M., & Jaroensutasinee, K. (2017). High motivation enables smaller contestants to win the contests in fiddler crabs (Brachyura, Ocypodidae). Crustaceana, 90(11–12), 1473–1481.
    Tricarico, E., & Gherardi, F. (2007). Resource assessment in hermit crabs: The worth of their own shell. Behavioral Ecology, 18(3), 615-620.
    Tricarico, E., & Gherardi, F. (2010). Past ownership makes crayfish more aggressive. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64, 575-581.
    Umbers, K. D. L., Osborne, L., & Keogh, J. S. (2012). The effects of residency and body size on contest initiation and outcome in the territorial dragon, Ctenophorus decresii. PLOS ONE, 7(10), e47143.
    Wilson, D. S., Coleman, K., Clark, A. B., & Biederman, L. (1993). Shy-bold continuum in pumpkinseed sunfish (Lepomis gibbosus): An ecological study of a psychological trait. Journal of comparative psychology, 107(3), 250.

    下載圖示
    QR CODE