研究生: |
林東昇 Lin, Dong-Sheng |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
探討多重表徵教學對八年級學生複習酸鹼鹽單元的學習成效與心智模式建立的影響 Investigating the Effectiveness of Multiple Representations Teaching on Eighth Graders’ Reviewing Performance and Established Mental Model of Acid-Bases-Salts |
指導教授: |
邱美虹
Chiu, Mei-Hung |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
科學教育研究所 Graduate Institute of Science Education |
論文出版年: | 2016 |
畢業學年度: | 104 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 159 |
中文關鍵詞: | 多重表徵 、酸與鹼 、心智模式 |
英文關鍵詞: | multiple representations, acid and base, mental model |
DOI URL: | https://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202204042 |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:125 下載:8 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
本研究旨在探討多重表徵教學法與傳統教學法對八年級學生在暑期輔導期間複習酸鹼鹽單元的學習成效與心智模式建立的影響。研究設計為準實驗研究設計,研究對象為新竹市某國中兩班學生共38人,其中實驗組學生有18人,對照組學生有20人,實驗組之教學方式採多重表徵教學法,並搭配筆者自行設計的學習單,對照組之教學方式採傳統板書的方式,並搭配市售的複習講義,教學時間為三堂課。
本研究結果顯示:
一、 多重表徵教學組學生在酸鹼概念診斷測驗的整體後測及延宕測表現都顯著
優於傳統教學組,而在三個次主題的分析中,在較著重記憶性知識的單元
裡,多重表徵教學法和傳統教學法的差異不大;但在較複雜的單元中,多重
表徵教學法的教學成效明顯優於傳統教學法。
二、 兩組學生在心智模式的改變上,多重表徵教學組的表現優於傳統教學組,
能夠有效地增進學生建構更良好的心智模式。
三、 在兩組的半結構式晤談方面,教學前兩組學生並無差異。而在教學後,
多重表徵組的回答正確比率比傳統教學組高出許多,並且傾向於利用粒子
等概念及動態的過程解釋相關概念,然而傳統教學組的學生在晤談當中仍
呈現許多不穩定性及不完整性,即便是高成就組的學生亦然。足以證明多
重表徵教學法較傳統教學法對於建構正確的酸鹼概念擁有良好的建設性。
四、 經由分析學生在學習情意問卷的想法,發現實驗組多數學生普遍認同多
重表徵教學法。
本研究建議:1、教科書在編排中可適度融入多重表徵的編排設計,利用多重表徵的教學策略去呈現科學知識中的微觀概念。2、針對較抽象的微觀概念,教師可以結合教師群組的力量共同設計多重表徵教學的教案與活動。
This study mainly focuses on the comparison between the application of Multiple Representations Teaching and Traditional Teaching to 8th-grade students on their reviewing performance as well as the effect on established mental models during the summer classes on the topic of Acids-Bases-Salts. Quasi-Experimental Design is adopted to conduct this research. The participants in this research includes 38 students from two classes, 18 students in the experimental group and 20 students in the control group respectively. The teaching method applied to the experimental group is Multiple Representations Teaching; moreover, the worksheets designed by the author of this study are applied as well. As for the control group, Traditional Teaching with commercial review course materials are applied. The teaching time is three classes.
The results of this study show:
1. In the diagnostic test of concepts related to acid, the experimental group outperforms the control group on the overall performance both in the post-test and the retention test. As for the three sub-topics, in the unit focusing more on memorizing, no significant difference is showed between the two methods. Nevertheless, in a more complex unit, Multiple Representations Teaching is evidently superior to Traditional Teaching.
2. In the two sets of the students’ mental models, the performance of experimental group surpasses the control group; besides, the Multiple Representations Teaching can effectively build up finer mental models.
3. Before the teaching, the interviews with every student show no difference between the two groups. Yet after teaching, the experimental group has a higher percentage to answer correctly than the control group. Also the students are able to explain the related concepts with particles concepts and dynamic process. However, in the interview with the control group, the students show more instability and incompleteness. Even the high achievers are no exception. Therefore, it is sufficient to prove that Multiple Representation Teaching can not only be more effective but more constructive in helping students build up a correct acid-base concept than Traditional Teaching Methods.
4. After the analysis of the questionnaires about learning motivation answered by the participants, most participants in the experimental group prefer Multiple Representations Teaching.
Suggestions for this research : 1. Adding a certain amount of Multiple Representations Teaching into textbooks and making use of teaching strategies of Multiple Representations to present microscopic concept of scientific knowledge.
2. For the more microscopic abstract concept, teachers can work together to design lesson plans and teaching activities of Multiple Representations.
一、中文部分
孔恩 ( T.Kuhn )著,王道還編譯(1989)。科學革命的結構,增訂新版。台北市:遠流出版社。
邱美虹(2000)。概念改變的省思與啟示。科學教育學刊,8(1),1–34。
宋志雄(1992)。探究國三學生酸與鹼迷思概念並應用以發展教學診斷工具(未出版之碩士論文)。國立彰化師範大學,彰化市。
李世峰(2002)。高中學生應用勒沙特列原理判斷非均勻相系化學平衡的迷思概念探討(未出版之碩士論文)。國立台灣師範大學,台北市。
姚錦棟(2002)。我國中學生酸鹼鹽迷思概念和心智模式之研究(未出版之碩士論
文)。國立台灣師範大學,台北市。
陳淑筠(2002)。國內學生自然科學迷思概念研究之後設研究(未出版之碩士論
文)。國立台東師範學院,台東市。
郭重吉(1988)。從認知觀點探討自然科學的學習。教育學院學報,13,351-378。
張春興(1989)。張氏心理學辭典。臺北市:東華書局。
曾永祥與許瑛玿(2006)。線上課程對高二學生四季成因概念學習的影響。科學教育學刊,14(3),257-282。
黃文田(2013)。探討建模教學對八年級學生酸鹼概念發展與建模能力的影響(未出版之碩士論文)。國立台灣師範大學,台北市。
黃萬居(1996)。國小教師對酸鹼的迷思概念之研究。臺北市立師範學院學報,27,105-132.
蔡俊義(2011)。多重表徵理論在理化科教學成效之研究—以酸鹼鹽單元為例(未出版之碩士論文)。國立臺灣師範大學,台北市。
鍾曉蘭(2007)。以多重表徵的模型教學探究高二學生理想氣體心智模式的類型及演變的途徑(未出版之碩士論文)。國立臺灣師範大學,台北市。
鍾曉蘭與邱美虹(2012)。高二學生在理想氣體多重表徵教學前後心智模式的改變。教育科學研究期刊, 57(4), 73-101
鍾聖校(1994):對科學教育錯誤概念研究之省思。教育研究資訊,2(3),89-110。
二、西文部分
Ainsworth, S.E (2006). DeFT: A conceptual framework for learning with multiple representations. Learning and Instruction, 16(3), 183-198.
Chi, M. T. H. (1992). Conceptual change within and across ontological categories:Implications for learning and discovery in sciences. In R. Giere (Ed.), Cognitive
models of science: Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science (pp.129-186).Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Chi, M.T.H. (2005). Common sense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14: 161-199.
Chi, M. T. H., Slotta, J. D., & deLeeuw, N. (1994). From things to processes: A theory of conceptual change for learning science concepts, Learning and instruction, 4, 27-43.
Chi, M. T. H., & Roscoe, R. D. (2002). The processes and challenges of conceptual change. In M. Limon and L. Mason (Eds.), Reconsidering Conceptual Change: Issues in Theory and Practice. Kluwer Academic Publishers, The Netherlands, pp 3-27.
Chi, M. T. H., & Hausmann, R. G. M. (2003). Do radical discoveries require ontological shifts? In L. Shavinina & R. Sternberg (Eds.) International Handbook on Innovation (Vol. 3, pp. 430–444). New York: Elsevier Science.
Chiu, M. H. (2007b). Research And InstructioN-Based/Oriented Work (RAINBOW) for Conceptual Change in Science Learning. Paper present at the second NICE
Symposium, July 30-31, Taipei, Taiwan.
Gilbert, J. K., & Watts, D. M. (1983).
Concepts,misconceptions and alternative conceptions:Changing perspectives in science education. Studies in science education, 10, 61-98.
Gussarsky, E. & Gorodetsky, M. (1990) . On the Concept “Chemical Equilibrium”:The Associative Framework. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, v27, n3,p197-204.
Johnstone, A.H. (1993). The development of chemistry teaching. Journal of Chemical Education , 70 (9), 701-705.
Johnson-Laird P. N. (1994). Mental models, Deductive Reasoning, and the Brain. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neural Science (pp. 999-1008). Cambridge: The MIT. Press.
Lin, J. W., & Chiu, M. H. (2007a). Exploring the Characteristics and Diverse sources of students’ Mental models of Acids and Bases. International Journal of Science Education, 29(6), 771-803.
Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., & Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: toward a theory of conceptual change. Science education, 66(2), 211-227.
Pfundt, F. & Duit, R. (1991). Bibliography: Students’ alternative frameworks and science education. (3rd ed.). Keil, West Germany: IPN.
Stavy, R. (1991). Children’s ideas about matter. School Science and Mathematics, 91,240-244.
Thagard, P. (1992). Conceptual revolutions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Vosniadou, S. (1994). Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change. [special issue]. Learning and instruction, 4, 45-69.
Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A study of
conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535-585.