研究生: |
林幸樺 Lin, Hsing-Hua |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
自我控制回饋的決定:知覺和實際表現的比較及其對動作表現與學習的效應 Decisions of Self-controlled Feedback: Comparisons of Perceived and Actual Performance and its Effects on Motor Performance and Learning |
指導教授: |
卓俊伶
Jwo, Jun-Ling |
口試委員: |
吳詩薇
Wu, Shih-Wei 梁嘉音 Liang, Chia-Yin 卓俊伶 Jwo, Jun-Ling |
口試日期: | 2021/06/29 |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
體育學系 Department of Physical Education |
論文出版年: | 2021 |
畢業學年度: | 109 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 44 |
中文關鍵詞: | 知覺判斷 、結果獲知 、自我控制回饋 |
英文關鍵詞: | perceived judgement, knowledge of results, self-controlled feedback |
研究方法: | 實驗設計法 、 準實驗設計法 |
DOI URL: | http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202101087 |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:119 下載:7 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
本研究旨在探討自我控制回饋情境下,學習者知覺與實際表現結果的差異,以及動作表現好與差之知覺判斷對動作表現與學習的影響。40位成人參與者,以非慣用手自我配速滑塊推動動作為實驗工作,進行獲得期60次與保留測驗12次的試作。以獲得期之參與者每次要求回饋後、接收回饋前,其回饋要求問題的填答次數,區分為自我控制回饋知覺好表現 (SC-good) 組或自我控制回饋知覺差表現 (SC-poor) 組。自變項為自我控制回饋知覺好與差表現,依變項為絕對誤差 (AE) 值及變異誤差 (VE) 值,獲得期以AE值與VE值進行2 (組別) × 6 (區間) 混合設計二因子變異數分析,其中區間為重複量數;而保留測驗的AE值與VE值則以單因子變異數進行分析。為瞭解兩組參與者自我控制回饋知覺表現是否與實際表現準確性差異,以兩組參與者在每區間要求回饋與未要求回饋之試作的AE值進行單因子變異數分析。結果發現:(一)SC-good組要求回饋之知覺好表現試作的AE值顯著低於未選擇回饋之試作,且SC-poor 組中有要求回饋與無要求回饋試作的AE值無顯著差異;(二)在獲得期及保留測驗中,SC-good組的AE值及VE值皆顯著低於SC-poor 組。本研究結論:(一)在自我控制回饋條件下,知覺好情境下的知覺表現與實際表現相同,而知覺差情境下的知覺表現與實際表現不相同;(二)在自我控制回饋情境下,對動作知覺好會促進動作表現與學習。
The purpose of this study was to explore the consistency between learners' perceived and actual performance in the context of self-controlled feedback, and the influence of perceived judgment of good and poor performance on motor performance and learning. Forty adults served as participants and experienced 60 trials during acquisition phase and 12 trials for retention test. Participants were divided into self-controlled feedback perceived good performance (SC-good) group or self-controlled feedback perceived poor performance (SC-poor) group, which was based the number of times they filled in the feedback request question. The independent variable was perceived good or poor performance of self-controlled feedback, and the dependent variables were absolute error (AE) and variable error (VE). The AE and VE during acquisition were analyzed by 2 (groups) × 6 (blocks) mixed design ANOVA with block as repeated measure; AE and VE in retention test were analyzed by one-way ANOVA. To examine the difference in accuracy between perceived and actual performance under self-controlled feedback, the AE of which trials they requested feedback and of which they didn’t requested feedback were analyzed by one-way ANOVA. The results showed that: (1) AE of the perceived good performance feedback in SC-good group were significantly lower than those trials which without requesting feedback, and there was no significant difference between AE with and without requesting feedback in SC-poor group; (2) In the acquisition phase and retention test, AE and VE in SC-good group were significantly lower than those in SC-poor group. It was concluded that (1) Under the condition of self-controlled feedback, the perceived performance in the context of good was the same with the actual performance, while the perceived performance in the context of poor was different from the actual performance and, (2) In the context of self-controlled feedback, perceived good on the performance facilitated motor performance and learning.
吳詩薇、張至滿、卓俊伶(2012)。摘要結果獲知的時間與順序安排:檢驗時近效應。大專體育學刊,14(2),152-160。https://doi.org/10.5297/ser.1402.002
吳詩薇、張至滿、卓俊伶、蔡銘仁(2016)。摘要結果獲知的時近訊息有利於自我控制學習。大專體育學刊,18(1),14-26。https://doi.org/10.5297/ser.1801.002
林清山(1992)。心理與教育統計學。東華。
林逸、卓俊伶(2016)。動作學習過程的自我效能變化。中華體育季刊,30(1),25-30。https://doi.org/10.3966/102473002016033001004
卓俊伶、楊梓楣(2017)。動作學習研究的新視窗:人因變項。大專體育學刊,19(1),i-vii。https://doi.org/10.5297/ser.1901.editorial
葉俊良、卓俊伶、林靜兒、陳重佑(2007)。自我控制回饋對動作表現、學習及錯誤估計的效應。大專體育學刊,9(1),23-35。https://doi.org/10.5297/ser.200703_9(1).0002
Aiken, C. A., Post, P. G., Hout, M. C., & Fairbrother, J. T. (2020). Self-controlled amount and pacing of practice facilitate learning of a sequential timing task. Journal of Sports Sciences, 38(4), 405-415. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2019.1704498
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. https://doi.org/10.1016/0146-6402(78)90002-4
Bandura, A. (2012). On the functional properties of perceived self-efficacy revisited. Journal of Management, 38(1), 9-44. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206311410606
Bandura, A., & Wood, R. (1989). Effect of perceived controllability and performance standards on self-regulation of complex decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 805-814. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.5.805
Barros, J. A. C., Yantha, Z. D., Carter, M. J., Hussien, J., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2019). Examining the impact of error estimation on the effects of self-controlled feedback. Human Movement Science, 63, 182-198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2018.12.002
Bilodeau, E. A., & Bilodeau, I. M. (1958). Variable frequency of knowledge of results and the learning of a simple skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(4), 379. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043214
Carter, M. J., Carlsen, A. N., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2014). Self-controlled feedback is effective if it is based on the learner’s performance: A replication and extension of Chiviacowsky and Wulf (2005). Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1325).
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01325
Carter, M. J., & Patterson, J. T. (2012). Self-controlled knowledge of results: Age-related differences in motor learning, strategies, and error detection. Human Movement Science, 31(6), 1459-1472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2012.07.008
Chen, D., Hendrick, J. L., & Lidor, R. (2002). Enhancing self-controlled learning environment: The use of self-regulated feedback information. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 43, 69-86.
Chen, D., & Singer, R. N. (1992). Self-regulation and cognitive strategies in sport participation. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23(4), 277-300.
Chiviacowsky, S. (2014). Self-controlled practice: Autonomy protects perceptions of competence and enhances motor learning. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15(5), 505-510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.05.003
Chiviacowsky, S., & Lessa, H. T. (2017). Choices over feedback enhance motor learning in older adults. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, 5(2), 304-318. https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0031
Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2002). Self-controlled feedback: Does it enhance learning because performers get feedback when they need it? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 73(4), 408-415. http://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2002.10609040
Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2005). Self-controlled feedback is effective if it is based on the learner's performance. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76(1), 42-48. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2005.10599260
Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., de Medeiros, F. L., Kaefer, A., & Tani, G. (2008). Learning benefits of self-controlled knowledge of results in 10-year-old children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79(3), 405-410. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2008.10599505
Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012). Self-controlled learning: The importance of protecting perceptions of competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 458. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00458
Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., Wally, R., & Borges, T. (2009). Knowledge of results after good trials enhances learning in older adults. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 80(3), 663-668. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2009.10599606
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dale H. Schunk (1995). Self-efficacy, motivation, and performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 7(2), 112-137. https://doi.org/10.1080/10413209508406961
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1024. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1024
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. N. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. Plenum Press.
Feltz, D. L. (1984). Self-efficacy as a cognitive mediator of athletic performance. In W. F. Straub & J. M. Williams (Eds.), Cognitive sport psychology, (pp.191-198). Sport Science Associates.
Janelle, C. M., Barba, D. A., Frehlich, S. G., Tennant, L. K., & Cauraugh, J. H. (1997). Maximizing performance feedback effectiveness through videotape replay and a self-controlled learning environment. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68(4), 269-279. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1997.10608008
Janelle, C. M., Kim, J., & Singer, R. N. (1995). Subject-controlled performance feedback and learning of a closed motor skill. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81(2), 627-634. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1995.81.2.627
Januário, M., Figueiredo, L., Portes, L., & Benda, R. (2019). Effects of self-controlled knowledge of results on learning a taekwondo serial skill. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 126(6), 1178-1194. https://doi.org/10.1177/0031512519869086
Ghorbani, S., & Bund, A. (2019). Motivational effects of enhanced expectancies for motor learning in individuals with high and low self-efficacy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 127(1), 263-274. https://doi.org/10.1177/0031512519892390
Maibach, E., & Murphy, D. A. (1995). Self-efficacy in health promotion research and practice: Conceptualization and measurement. Health Education Research, 10(1), 37-50. https://doi.org/10.1093/her/10.1.37
Oldfiled, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburg inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-112. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Saemi, E., Porter, J. M., Ghotbi-Varzaneh, A., Zarghami, M., & Maleki, F. (2012). Knowledge of results after relatively good trials enhances self-efficacy and motor learning. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(4), 378-382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.12.008
Salmoni, A. W., Schmidt, R. A., & Walter, C. B. (1984). Knowledge of results and motor learning: A review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 355-386. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.355
Schmidt, R. A. (1991). Frequent augmented feedback can degrade learning: Evidence and interpretations. Tutorials in motor neuroscience (pp. 59-75). Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Schmidt, R. A., & Bjork, R. A. (1992). New conceptualizations of practice: Common
principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3, 207-217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992. tb00029.x
Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2011). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis (5th ed.). Human Kinetics.
Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2013). Motor learning and performance: From principles to application (5th ed.). Human Kinetics.
Tsai, M. J., & Jwo, H. (2015). Controlling absolute frequency of feedback in a self-controlled situation enhances motor learning. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 121(3), 746-758. https://doi.org/10.2466/23.PMS.121c28x7
Vallerand, R. J. (2002). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: A hierarchical model. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds), Handbook of self-determination research (pp.37-63). University of Rochester Press.
Vieira, M., Ugrinowitsch, H., Gallo, L., Carvalho, M., Fonseca, M., & Benda, R. (2014). Effects of summary knowledge of results in motor skills acquisition. Revista de Psicologia del Deporte, 23, 9-14.
Wehmeyer, M., Hughes, C., Agran, M., Garner, N., & Yeager, D. (2003). Student‐directed learning strategies to promote the progress of students with intellectual disability in inclusive classrooms. International Journal of Inclusive Education, 7(4), 415-428.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1360311032000110963
Winstein, C. J., & Schmidt, R. A. (1990). Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(4), 677. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.677
Wrisberg, C. A., & Pein, R. L. (2002). Note on learners’ control of the frequency of model presentation during skill acquisition. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 792-794. https://doi.org/10.2466%2Fpms.2002.94.3.792
Wulf, G., Chiviacowsky, S., & Cardozo, P. L. (2014). Additive benefits of autonomy support and enhanced expectancies for motor learning. Human Movement Science, 37, 12-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2014.06.004
Wulf, G., Chiviacowsky, S., & Drews, R. (2015). External focus and autonomy support: Two important factors in motor learning have additive benefits. Human Movement Science, 40, 176-184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2014.11.015
Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2016). Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(5), 1382-1414. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0999-9
Zimmerman, B. J. (1990). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: An overview. Educational Psychologist, 25(1), 3-17. https://doi.org/10.1207/ s15326985ep2501_2