研究生: |
林郁良 Yu-liang Lin |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
高中英文衍生字尾知識之研究 A Study on Taiwanese Senior High School Students' Knowledge of English Derivational Suffixes |
指導教授: |
程玉秀
Cheng, Yuh-Show |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
英語學系 Department of English |
論文出版年: | 2011 |
畢業學年度: | 100 |
語文別: | 英文 |
論文頁數: | 141 |
中文關鍵詞: | 衍生字尾 、產出性知識 、認識性知識 |
英文關鍵詞: | derivational suffix, productive knowledge, receptive knowledge |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:152 下載:26 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
本研究旨在調查台灣高三學生對英文衍生字尾的產出性知識(productive knowledge)與認識性知識(receptive knowledge),依照正確產出與認識的英文衍生字尾排出順序,並計算產出性知識與認識性知識分別和受試者的字彙量及整體英文程度的相關性。
受試者為170名某完全中學的高三生。研究者從學校方面取得其學測英文成績,據以判定其整體英文程度。為檢測其字彙量,受試者需接受字彙程度測驗(VLT)。此外,產出性衍生字尾測驗(PT)用以評估其對衍生字尾的產出性知識,認識性衍生字尾測驗(RT)則是衡量其對衍生字尾的認識性知識。在產出性衍生字尾測驗中,針對20個字根受試者需依4種詞性(名詞、形容詞、動詞、副詞)寫出合適的衍生字。在認識性衍生字尾測驗中,受試者會見到同樣的20個字根,但不同的是,其後會有一列可能與其連用的字尾,受試者需將能和這些字根連用的字尾圈出來。
產出性衍生字尾測驗的結果顯示,前五個最常被使用到的名詞字尾是-ion/-tion、-ation、-er/-or、-ity、-ic,最常被使用到的形容詞字尾是-ive、-al、-able/-ible、-ic和-ful。總共只有3個動詞衍生字尾(-ize、-fy、-ate)、僅有1個副詞衍生字尾(-ly)被所有170名受試者使用到。若不論詞性,則-ly、-able/-ible、-er/-or和-al是最多受試者答案中用到的字尾。而出現次數最多的,則是-ion/-tion、-ly和-ive。從認識性衍生字尾測驗的結果,可排列出正確認識率由高至低的字尾: -ship > -ary > -ure > -ity > -ize > -ic > -fy > -ful > -ation > -ly > -less > -ion > -ive > -al > -able > -er/-or。就產出性衍生字尾測驗和認識性衍生字尾測驗來說,受試者在前者的表現顯著低於後者。而對受試者的字彙量、整體英文程度、衍生性字尾的產出性知識、衍生性字尾的認識性知識四者的相關性研究顯示,這四者彼此之間呈現顯著正相關。
由於本研究結果發現,衍生字尾和學測英文成績及整體英文程度關係密切,故對高中生來說,英文衍生字尾知識應是重要且適合的教學目標。為幫助學生學習英文衍生字尾,教師首先可根據Nation的「字尾次序表」,由常見且高頻率的衍生字尾開始,將字尾介紹給學生,讓學生認識字彙中出現的衍生字尾並且瞭解其意義;教師也應教導學生衍生字尾所代表的詞性。此外,在教導新單字時,教師可連同其字族一併教給學生,以幫助學生處理字義難理解的單字,並使學生注意到字彙形成的規則及過程,俾使學生逐漸養成字首、字尾、複合字的概念。高中生應被教導如何產出字根或字尾的拼法出現改變的衍生字。至於字尾可跟哪些字根連用的限制,這種知識則不要教給一般程度的台灣高中生,但可教給程度較好的學生,如英語資優生。
The present study aimed to investigate Taiwanese twelfth graders’ productive and receptive English derivational suffix knowledge. The accuracy orders of English derivational suffix production and recognition were established; the correlations between both suffix knowledge and vocabulary size as well as general English proficiency were calculated.
The subjects were 170 twelfth graders from a complete school. From the school authorities the researcher obtained the subjects’ SAET scores, which were gathered to determine their general English proficiency. Then the Vocabulary Levels Test (VLT) was administered to measure the subjects’ vocabulary size while the Productive Derivational Suffix Test (PT) and the Receptive Derivational Suffix Test (RT) were designed to assess their productive and receptive suffix knowledge. In the PT, the subjects were given twenty prompt words and required to produce appropriate derivatives corresponding to four word classes, namely noun, adjective, verb, and adverb. In the RT, the test items were the same as those in the PT, except that a list of possible suffixes followed each prompt, from which allowable suffixes were to be circled.
The results of the PT show that the top five productive suffixes in noun were -ion/-tion, -ation, -er/-or, -ity, and -ic, whereas those in adjective were -ive, -al, -able/-ible, -ic, and -ful. However, simply three verbal derivational suffixes (-ize, -fy, and -ate) were supplied by all the subjects and only one adverbial suffix, -ly, contributed to the formation of adverbs. Irrespective of parts of speech, -ly, -able/-ible, -er/-or, and -al were the most productive suffixes in terms of variety. In terms of frequency, the most productive suffixes became -ion/-tion, followed by -ly and -ive. The findings of the RT helped to establish an accuracy order of derivational suffix recognition: -ship > -ary > -ure > -ity > -ize > -ic > -fy > -ful > -ation > -ly > -less > -ion > -ive > -al > -able > -er/-or. The subjects’ performance in the PT was significantly worse than that in the RT. The correlation results among vocabulary size, general English proficiency, productive suffix knowledge, and receptive suffix knowledge show that the four measures were positively and significantly correlated with one another.
The findings of the current study suggest that derivational suffixes might be an important and appropriate target of instruction for senior high school students because of its close relationship with the SAET score and general English ability. To help students learn the derivational suffixes, a selected number of derivational suffixes should first be introduced to students and Nation’s (2001) Sequenced List provides a useful set of learning goals. The teaching and learning of derivational suffixes should start from the widely-attached, frequently-occurring ones. Next, learners should be taught to recognize the suffixes in words and to learn the meanings of these suffixes. Time and effort should also be devoted to the teaching of suffixes’ syntactic functions, especially the word class of the suffix. English teachers could consider including other members of the same word family in the teaching plan when introducing new words, which may assist students in dealing with semantically opaque words. Besides, teaching word families may attract students’ attention to word formation regularities and processes, which can develop the concepts of prefixed words and compounds along with those of suffixed words. High school students should also be specifically instructed to produce derivatives whose stem or suffix undergoes orthographic changes. However, it is advised to provide distributional knowledge only for Taiwanese senior high school students of a higher level of proficiency in English, such as the gifted and talented students.
Agnes, M. E. (Ed.). (2004). Webster’s New World College Dictionary. (4th ed.). New York: Macmillan.
Anderson, R. C., & Nagy, W. E. (1993). The vocabulary conundrum (Technical Report No. 570). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL: Center for the Study of Reading.
Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58. 1-166.
Bauer, L., & Nation, I. S. P. (1993). Word families. International Journal of Lexicography, 6, 253-279.
Beglar, D., & Hunt, A. (1999). Revising and validating the 2000 Word Level and University Word Level Vocabulary Tests. Language Testing, 16, 131-162.
Bullon, S. (Ed.). (2009). Longman Dictionary of Contemporary English (5th ed.). London: Longman.
Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247-266.
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 169-190.
Carlisle, J. F. (2003). Morphology matters in learning to read: A commentary. Reading Psychology, 24, 291-322.
Carlisle, J. F., & Fleming, J. (2003). Lexical processing of morphologically complex words in the elementary years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 239-253.
Carlisle, J. F., & Katz, L. A. (2006). Effects of word and morpheme familiarity on reading of derived words. Reading and Writing, 19, 669-693.
Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (2005). Exploring the role of morphemes in word reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 4. 428-449.
Champion, A. (1997). Knowledge of suffixed words: A comparison of reading disabled and nondisabled readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 29-55.
Chang, C. Y. (2007). A study on the effects of vocabulary learning strategy instruction on vocabulary learning of senior high school students in Taiwan. Unpublished master thesis, National Taiwan Normal University, Taiwan.
Chiang, W. C. (2008). A morphological approach via commonly-used roots to French vocabulary teaching: How to help Taiwanese students memorize French vocabulary. Unpublished master thesis, Fu Jen Catholic University, Taiwan.
Ciou, S. R. (2008). A study on the analysis of Taiwanese EFL learners’ acquisition of English derivational morphological rules. Unpublished master thesis, National Kaohsiung Normal University, Taiwan.
De Jong, N. H., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2000). The morphological family size effect and morphology. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 329-365.
Dell, G. S. (1990). Effects of frequency and vocabulary type on phonological speech errors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5, 313-349.
Feldman, L. B. (1991). The contribution of morphology to word recognition. Psychological Research, 53, 33-41.
Freyd, P., & Baron, J. (1982). Individual differences in acquisition of derivational morphology. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 282-295.
Hancin-Bhatt, B., & Nagy, W. (1993). Bilingual students’ developing understanding of morphologically complex cognates (Technical report No. 567). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL: Center for the Study of Reading.
Hayashi, Y., & Murphy, V. A. (2009). Morphological awareness and second language receptive and productive vocabulary knowledge in Japanese learners of English. Paper presented at the International Conference on First and Second Languages: Exploring the Relationship in Pedagogy-Related Context, University of Oxford.
Henriksen, B. (1996). Semantisation, retention, and accessibility: Key concepts in vocabulary learning. Paper presented at the AILA Congress, Jyvaskyla, Finland, as cited in Hayashi, Y., & Murphy, V. A. (2009). Morphological awareness and second language receptive and productive vocabulary knowledge in Japanese learners of English. Paper presented at the International Conference on First and Second Languages: Exploring the Relationship in Pedagogy-Related Context, University of Oxford.
Ho, Y. C. (2009). Relationships between college English majors’ abilities in academic vocabulary and its derivatives of word family. Unpublished master thesis, National Kaohsiung Normal University, Taiwan.
Hornby, A. S. (Ed.). (1995). Oxford Advanced Learner’s Dictionary of Current English (5th ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hsu, C. F. (2004). A study of the effect of word analysis instruction on vocabulary acquisition in a Taiwanese senior high school. Unpublished master thesis, National Chengchi University, Taiwan.
Hsu, K. F. (2004). An iconic-morphological approach via commonly-used roots to English vocabulary teaching: How to help Chinese senior high school students memorize English vocabulary. Unpublished master thesis, National Chengchi University, Taiwan.
Huang, M. U. (2007). Acquisition of English derivational patterns: Integrating word sorting with phonological awareness training. Unpublished master thesis, National Changhua University of Education, Taiwan.
Ivimey, G. P. (1975). The development of English morphology: An acquisition model. Language and Speech, 18, 120-144.
Jarmulowicz, L. (2006). School-aged children’s phonological production of derived English words. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 294-308.
Jarmulowicz, L., & Taran, V. L. (2007). Exploration of lexical-semantic factors affecting stress production in derived words. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 378-389.
Kao, H. S. (2007). A study on Taiwanese high school students’ Chinese and English morphological awareness. Unpublished master thesis, National Chung Cheng University, Taiwan.
Ke, H. C. (2005). Effects of derivational suffix learning on senior high school students. Unpublished master thesis, National Kaohsiung Normal University, Taiwan.
Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2007). Breaking down words to build meaning: Morphology, vocabulary, and reading comprehension in the urban classroom. The Reading Teacher, 61, 134-144.
Krott, A., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (1999). Complex words in complex words. Linguistics, 37, 905-926.
Ku, Y., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading and Writing: An interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 399-422.
Kuo, L. J., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: a cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180.
Laufer, B. (1998). The development of passive and active vocabulary in a second language: Same or different? Applied Linguistics, 19, 255-271.
Laufer, B., & Paribakht, T. S. (1998). The relationship between passive and active vocabularies: Effects of language learning context. Language Learning, 48, 365-391.
Leong, C. K. (2000). Rapid processing of root and derived forms of words and grades 4, 5 and 6 children’s spelling. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 277-302.
Lewis, D. J., & Windsor, J. (1996). Children’s analysis of derivational suffix meanings. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 39, 209-216.
Li, E. L. (1998). A study of the effects of word analysis on vocabulary teaching and learning in senior high school. Unpublished master thesis. National Taiwan Normal University, Taiwan.
Luo, Z. J. (2008). A study of the effects of derivational suffix instruction on EFL majors’ morphological knowledge. Unpublished master thesis, Husan Chuang University, Taiwan.
Mahony, D. (1994). Using sensitivity to word structure to explain variance in high school and college level reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 19-44.
Mahony, D., Singson, M., & Mann, V. (2000). Reading ability and sensitivity to morphological relations. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 191-218.
Melinger, A. (2001). The contribution of semantic transparency to the morphological decomposition of prefixed words. Folia Linguistica, 35, 285-297.
Mochizuki, M., & Aizawa, K. (2001). An affix acquisition order for EFL learners: An exploratory study. System, 28, 291-304.
Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304-330.
Nagy, W. E., Diakidoy, I. A., & Anderson, R. C. (1993). The acquisition of morphology: Learning the contribution of suffixes to the meanings of derivatives. Journal of Reading Behavior, 23, 155-170.
Nation, I. S. P. (1990). Teaching and learning vocabulary. Boston: Heinle & Heinle Publishers.
Nation, I. S. P. (1996). Vocabulary lists. English Language Institute Occasional Publication No. 17. Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington.
Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Procter, P. (Ed.). (1995). Cambridge International Dictionary of English (5th ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Read, J. (2000). Assessing vocabulary. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Reichle, E. D., & Perfetti, C. A. (2003). Morphology in word identification: A word-experience model that accounts for morpheme frequency effects. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 219-237.
Ruan, J. C. (2008). The relationships among affix knowledge, syntactic knowledge and vocabulary size in Taiwan college students’ English. Unpublished master thesis, Providence University, Taiwan.
Schmitt, N. (1998). Tracking the incremental acquisition of second language vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Language Learning, 48, 281-317.
Schmitt, N. (2000). Vocabulary in language teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schmitt, N., & Meara, P. (1997). Researching vocabulary through a word knowledge framework: Word associations and verbal suffixes. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 17-36.
Schmitt, N., Schmitt, D., & Clapham, C. (2001). Developing and exploring the behavior of two new versions of the Vocabulary Levels Test. Language Testing, 18, 55-88.
Schmitt, N., & Zimmerman, C. B. (2002). Derivative word forms: What do learners know? TESOL Quarterly, 36, 145-171.
Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (1997). How complex simplex words can be. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 118-139.
Shih, W. H. (2002). The effects of applying phonics instruction and word analysis to vocabulary teaching in EFL classes. Unpublished master thesis, National Chengchi University, Taiwan.
Sinclair, J. (Ed.). (1995). Collins COBUILD English Dictionary for Advanced Learners (2nd ed.). London: Harper Collins.
Singson, M., Mahony, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 219-252.
Soanes, C. (Ed.). (2005). Compact Oxford English Dictionary of Current English (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stockwell, R., & Minkova, D. (2001). English words: History and structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Summers, D. (Ed.). (1992). Longman Dictionary of English Language and Culture (1st ed.). Harlow: Longman.
Taft, M. (1988). A morphological-decomposition model of lexical representation. Linguistics, 26, 657-667.
Taft, M. (2004). Morphological decomposition and the reverse root frequency effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 745-756.
Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. E. (1985). The role of derivational suffixes in sentence comprehension (Technical Report No. 357). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL: Center for the Study of Reading.
Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. E. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649-667.
Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. E. (1990). Use of derivational morphology during reading. Cognition, 36, 17-34.
Windsor, J. (1994). Children’s comprehension and production of derivational suffixes. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 37. 408-417.
Windsor, J. (2000). The role of phonological opacity in reading achievement. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 50-61.
Windsor, J., & Hwang, M. (1999). Derivational suffix productivity for students with and without language-learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 220-230.
Winsor, P., Nagy, W. E., Osborn, J., & O’Flahavan, J. (1993). Structural analysis: Toward an evaluation of instruction (Technical Report No. 581). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL: Center for the Study of Reading.
White, T. G., Sowell, J., & Yanagihara, A. (1989). Teaching elementary students to use word part clues. The Reading Teacher, 42, 302-308.
Wysocki, K., & Jenkins, J. R. (1987). Deriving word meanings through morphological generalization. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 66-81.
大考中心高中英文參考詞彙表研究計畫小組編著(2002)。大學入學考試中心高中英文參考詞彙表。大學入學考試中心。民91年6月30日,取自:http://
www.ceec.edu.tw/Research/paper_doc/ce37/4.pdf