研究生: |
張俊文 Chang, Chun-Wen |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
翡翠樹蛙對環境中聲音訊息之行為反應 Behavioral response of acoustic information in Rhacophorus prasinatus |
指導教授: |
林思民
Lin, Si-Min |
學位類別: |
博士 Doctor |
系所名稱: |
生命科學系 Department of Life Science |
論文出版年: | 2019 |
畢業學年度: | 107 |
語文別: | 英文 |
論文頁數: | 123 |
中文關鍵詞: | Alternative mating tactics 、Calling strategy 、Energy constraint 、Honest signal 、Noise masking 、Resource partition 、Satellite males |
英文關鍵詞: | Alternative mating tactics, Calling strategy, Energy constraint, Honest signal, Noise masking, Resource partition, Satellite males |
DOI URL: | http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU201900986 |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:155 下載:20 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
訊息傳遞在兩棲類的繁殖中扮演非常重要的角色,不管是物種辨識、挑選配偶或建立領域,都仰賴從週遭環境中獲取並判斷不同形式的訊息,並依判讀的資訊做出進一步的決定。聲音訊號是無尾目物種最常用來傳遞訊息的模式,雄蛙於繁殖季節聚集至水邊,以鳴叫聲吸引雌蛙前來交配。然而,周圍同種及異種個體所發出的鳴叫聲,甚至自然環境產生的噪音,都對鳴聲的傳遞造成干擾。這些逆境對動物來說是必須克服的挑戰,並可能是促使物種演化的原動力。鳴叫對雄蛙而言是非常耗能的行為,在噪音干擾之下,雄蛙應該具有相對應的行為策略,減少訊號傳遞時受到的干擾,並提高鳴聲傳遞的成功率,以獲得繁殖的機會。不過相關研究多為人工環境下之測試,或探討人為活動所產生的噪音干擾,而在自然狀況下針對雄蛙對共域異種聲音訊號之行為反應實驗則非常缺乏。我的研究目的是了解雄蛙在繁殖季中如何克服噪音所造成的干擾,並且針對雄蛙”如何尋找適合鳴叫的地點”,以及”加入鳴叫集團後的策略”這兩個角度探討雄性翡翠樹蛙(Rhacophorus prasinatus)在噪音干擾下的行為策略。第1章首先探討環境中噪音的種類,以及前人研究中動物對噪音的因應方式。第2章則使用聲音回播的方式,測試雄性翡翠樹蛙對不同回播聲音的趨聲偏好(Phonotaxis preference)。有別於前人在室內進行短距離的研究,我模擬翡翠樹蛙在自然環境的實際尺度,建置一個直徑20公尺的試驗場地進行測試,並且檢測以下4個問題:(1)雄蛙偏好加入大型或小型的合唱集團?(2)雄蛙偏好加入高品質或低品質的鳴叫同伴?(3)雄蛙是否利用異種鳴聲定位潛在的繁殖地點?(4)雄蛙是否會加入或避開具有異種鳴聲的混種合唱集團?結果顯示,雄性翡翠樹蛙偏好加入大型的合唱集團;低品質的雄性翡翠樹蛙傾向於接近高品質的雄性翡翠樹蛙,這可能屬於衛星雄性的潛行(Sneak)行為;此外,在沒有同種叫聲的狀況下,雄性翡翠樹蛙會利用生態棲位差異較大的腹斑蛙(Babina adenopleura)的叫聲尋找潛在繁殖地點,但不會接近使用生態棲位相似的布氏樹蛙(Polypedates braueri)叫聲,這也代表雄性翡翠樹蛙有能力透過鳴聲辨識共域物種。第3章我同樣使用聲音回播的方式,以分析雄蛙鳴聲反應的差異,探討雄性翡翠樹蛙對背景噪音干擾的因應策略,並且檢測以下5個問題:(1)雄蛙是否能夠調整節奏或鳴叫時機以避免鳴聲與噪音重疊?(2)雄性對不同音頻(Hz)噪音的反應為何?(3)雄蛙是否會加入具有相同音頻噪音的合唱集團?(4)不同品質的雄蛙在高強度噪音干擾下是否採取不同的鳴叫策略?(5)野外實際狀況下雄蛙的鳴叫反應為何?結果顯示,雄性翡翠樹蛙會利用噪音發出的空檔調整其鳴叫時機,避免鳴聲與噪音重疊;雄性翡翠樹蛙顯著避免於與自身鳴聲主頻率(~1500Hz)相同的中頻噪聲(1000Hz-2000Hz)時段鳴叫,並避開具有這類噪音的合唱集團;雖然在測試期間所有雄蛙其鳴叫率(call rate)均顯著下降,推測可能是為了在噪音干擾下節省自身能量,然而,與高品質雄性翡翠樹蛙相比,低品質量雄性的鳴叫率降低程度更為明顯,這也代表面對噪音干擾時所採取之鳴叫策略可能與雄性個體的身體狀況有關。在最後一個章節中,我針對研究結果中未來可延伸的議題進行探討,包括:物種鳴聲的主頻率可能為影響物種共存的另一種資源型態;噪音干擾可能促進訊號的演化;雄蛙在無法預測雌蛙出現的狀況下,將能量投資於繁殖場的出席率和投資於鳴叫展示,可能是權衡(trade-off)下的結果;物種的趨聲行為可應用在物種保育,以及外來種移除措施。本論文的研究結果不僅提供了第一個模擬自然尺度下的測試案例,亦同時檢測多物種的掩蔽效應(mask effect),並顯示在野外實際狀況下,蛙類的群集合唱行為可能為包含訊號傳遞、感官靈敏度、合唱團物種組成以及與競爭者之間敵我評估的綜合策略。
Acoustic communication plays a crucial role in the reproduction of most anuran amphibians, such as species recognition, mate choice and territory defense. For the species which aggregate in dense choruses, the high level of background noises would lead the frogs to face the interruption and further constrain their communication. Therefore, noise interference is a ubiquitous challenge for these animals. Signalers should behave to reduce masking by noise, and these strategies are presumably preferred by natural selection or sexual selection. The aim of my study was to explore how male frogs deal with background noises in the breeding season. By using broadcasting tests on a polyandrous choral treefrog Rhacophorus prasinatus, I examined how male frogs use conspecific and heterospecific signals to locate a chorus in Chapter 2 and tested the following questions: (1) Do male frogs prefer to join a large or a small chorus? (2) Do male frogs prefer to approach a high-quality or a low-quality male? (3) Do males utilize heterospecific calls to locate the potential breeding sites? (4) Do male frogs orient towards or avoid choruses containing heterospecific calls? My results indicated that male frogs prefer to join a large rather than small chorus. Low-quality males tend to approach high-quality males, which might be explained as the sneaking behavior of satellite males. Furthermore, male frogs may use heterospecific calls delivered by a noncompetitive sympatric species (a ranid frog) to find potential breeding sites, but not by a sympatric competitor (another rhacophorid frog) that occupies a similar niche. On the other hand, I further examined the response of males with the interference from background noise after join a chorus in Chapter 3, and aimed to answer the following questions: (1) Are male frogs able to adjust their tempo or call timing to avoid overlap with noise? (2) What is the response of males to noises with different frequencies? (3) Would male frogs choose to join or keep away from a chorus with too much noise interference? (4) Do males with different body conditions apply different strategies when facing high-intensity interference? (5) What is the strategy of males applied in the wild? The results indicated that male frogs adjusted their calling pattern by using the spacing of the noises to avoid a direct temporal overlapping and avoided medium-frequency noises which obviously overlapped their own signal after join chorus. Although all the frogs showed a significant decrease of call rates during broadcasting, possibly aiming to save their own energy; yet the level of decrease was more pronounced in low-quality males compared to high-quality ones. The results in my dissertation not only provided the first experimental test in natural condition which consider the multiple species masking effect in playback experiment, but also provided a guide for further approach, which should combine all relevant factors including signal transmission, sensory sensitivity, chorus joining decision, and alternative courtship strategy when testing the phonotaxis preference of anurans.
CH 1
Amézquita A, Flechas SV, Lima AP, Gasser H, Hodl W (2011) Acoustic interference and recognition space within a complex assemblage of dendrobatid frogs. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:17058–17063.
Barber JR, Crooks KR, Fristrup KM (2010) The costs of chronic noise exposure for terrestrial organisms. Trends Ecol Evol 25:180–189.
Bee MA (2007) Selective phonotaxis by male wood frogs (Rana sylvatica) to the sound of a chorus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61:955–966.
Bee MA, Schwartz JJ, Summers K (2013) All's well that begins Wells: celebrating 60 years of Animal Behaviour and 36 years of research on anuran social behaviour. Anim Behav 85:5–18.
Bleach IT, Beckmann C, Both C, Brown GP, Shine R (2015) Noisy neighbours at the frog pond: effects of invasive cane toads on the calling behaviour of native Australian frogs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 69:675–683.
Brepson L, Voituron Y, Lengagne T (2012) Condition-dependent ways to manage acoustic signals under energetic constraint in a tree frog. Behav Ecol 24:488–496.
Brumm H, Zollinger SA (2011) The evolution of the Lombard effect: 100 years of psychoacoustic research. Behaviour.148:1173–1198.
Buxton VL, Ward MP, Sperry JH (2018) Evaluation of Conspecific Attraction as a Management Tool across Several Species of Anurans. Diversity 10:6.
Cunnington GM, Fahrig L (2013) Mate attraction by male anurans in the presence of traffic noise. Anim Conserv 16:275–285.
Farine DR, Garroway CJ, Sheldon BC (2012) Social network analysis of mixed-species flocks: exploring the structure and evolution of interspecific social behaviour. Anim Behav. 84:1271–1277.
Feng AS, Ratnam R (2000) Neural basis of hearing in real-world situations. Annu Rev Psychol 51: 699–725.
Friedl TW, Klump GM (2005) Sexual selection in the lek-breeding European treefrog: body size, chorus attendance, random mating and good genes. Anim Behav. 70:1141–1154.
Gerhardt HC, Huber F (2002) Acoustic communication in insects and anurans: Common problems and diverse solutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Gerhardt HC, Klump GM (1988) Masking of acoustic signals by the chorus background noise in the green tree frog: A limitation on mate choice. Anim Behav. 36:1247–1249.
Grafe T, Thein J (2001) Energetics of calling and metabolic substrate use during prolonged exercise in the European treefrog Hyla arborea. J Comp Psychol 171:69–76.
Hanna DE, Wilsoon DR, Blouin-Demers G, Mennill DJ (2014) Spring peepers Pseudacris crucifer modify their call structure in response to noise. Curr Zool 60: 438–448.
Halfwerk W, Lea AM, Guerra MA, Page RA, Ryan MJ (2016) Vocal responses to noise reveal the presence of the Lombard effect in a frog. Behav Ecol 27:669–676.
Höbel G (2014) Socially mediated plasticity of chorusing behavior in the gladiator frog Hypsiboas rosenbergi. Acta Ethol 18:145–152.
Höbel G, Barta T (2014) Adaptive plasticity in calling site selection in grey treefrogs (Hyla versicolor). Behaviour 151:741–754.
Humfeld SC (2013) Condition-dependent signaling and adoption of mating tactics in an amphibian with energetic displays. Behav Ecol 24:859–870.
James MS, Stockwell MP, Clulow J, Clulow S, Mahony MJ (2015) Investigating behaviour for conservation goals: Conspecific call playback can be used to alter amphibian distributions within ponds. Biol Conserv 192:287–293.
Kishi S, Nakazawa T (2013) Analysis of species coexistence co-mediated by resource competition and reproductive interference. Popul Ecol 55:305–313.
Luther D, Gentry K (2013) Sources of background noise and their influence on vertebrate acoustic communication. Behaviour 150:1045–1068.
Mönkkönen M, Helle P, Niemi GJ, Montgomery K (1997) Heterospecific attraction affects community structure and migrant abundances in northern breeding bird communities. Can J Zool 75:2077–2083.
Magrath RD, Haff TM, Fallow PM, Radford AN (2015) Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: from mechanisms to consequences. Biol Rev Camb Philos 90:560–586.
Miller CW, Fletcher RJ, Jr., Gillespie SR (2013) Conspecific and heterospecific cues override resource quality to influence offspring production. PLoS One 8:e70268.
Mullet TC, Farina A, Gage SH (2017) The Acoustic Habitat Hypothesis: An Ecoacoustics Perspective on Species Habitat Selection. Biosemiotics 10:319–336.
Neelon DP, Höbel G (2017) Social plasticity in choosiness in green tree frogs, Hyla cinerea. Behav Ecol 28:1540–1546.
Ruppe L, Clement G, Herrel A, Ballesta L, Decamps T, Kever L, Parmentier E (2015) Environmental constraints drive the partitioning of the soundscape in fishes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:6092-7
Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD, Taft LK (1981) The costs and benefits of frog chorusing behavior. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 8:273–278.
Schwartz JJ, Hunce R, Lentine B, Powers K (2015) Calling site choice and its impact on call degradation and call attractiveness in the gray treefrog, Hyla versicolor. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 70:1–19.
Smith JM, Harper D (2003) Animal signals. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Toledo LF, Martins IA, Bruschi DP, Passos MA, Alexandre C, Haddad CFB (2014) The anuran calling repertoire in the light of social context. Acta Ethol 18:87–99.
Velez A, Bee MA (2010) Signal recognition by frogs in the presence of temporally fluctuating chorus-shaped noise. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:1695–1709.
Wells KD (2010) The ecology and behavior of amphibians. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Wiley, R.H. (2006). Signal detection and animal communication. Adv. Stud. Behav. 36: 217-247.
CH2
Amézquita A, Flechas SV, Lima AP, Gasser H, Hodl W (2011) Acoustic interference and recognition space within a complex assemblage of dendrobatid frogs. P Natl Acad Sci USA 108:17058–17063
Beckers OM, Schul J (2004) Phonotaxis in Hyla versicolor (Anura, Hylidae): the effect of absolute call amplitude. J Comp Physiol A 190:869–876
Bee MA (2007) Selective phonotaxis by male wood frogs (Rana sylvatica) to the sound of a chorus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61:955–966
Bee MA, Riemersma KK (2008) Does common spatial origin promote the auditory grouping of temporally separated signal elements in grey treefrogs? Anim Behav 76:831–843
Bee MA, Schwartz JJ, Summers K (2013) All's well that begins Wells: celebrating 60 years of Animal Behaviour and 36 years of research on anuran social behaviour. Anim Behav 85:5–18
Bee MA, Velez A, Forester JD (2012) Sound level discrimination by gray treefrogs in the presence and absence of chorus-shaped noise. J Acoust Soc Am 131:4188–4195
Bleach IT, Beckmann C, Both C, Brown GP, Shine R (2015) Noisy neighbours at the frog pond: effects of invasive cane toads on the calling behaviour of native Australian frogs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 69:675–683
Brepson L, Voituron Y, Lengagne T (2012) Condition-dependent ways to manage acoustic signals under energetic constraint in a tree frog. Behav Ecol 24:488–496
Buxton VL, Ward MP, Sperry JH (2018) Evaluation of conspecific attraction as a management tool across several species of Anurans. Diversity 10:6
Buxton VL, Ward MP, Sperry JH (2015) Use of chorus sounds for location of breeding habitat in 2 species of anuran amphibians. Behav Ecol 26:1111–1118
Chen S-L (1992) The reproductive behavior and ecology of emerald green tree frog (Rhacophorus smaragdinus). MSc thesis, National Taiwan Normal University
Chen T-Y (2002) Male advertisement call and female choice in Moltrecht's treefrog, Rhacophorus moltrechti, from Taiwan. MSc thesis, National Cheng Kung University
Cunnington GM, Fahrig L (2013) Mate attraction by male anurans in the presence of traffic noise. Anim Conserv 16:275–285
Diego-Rasilla FJ, Luengo RM (2004) Heterospecific call recognition and phonotaxis in the orientation behavior of the marbled newt, Triturus marmoratus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:556–560
Farine DR, Garroway CJ, Sheldon BC (2012) Social network analysis of mixed-species flocks: exploring the structure and evolution of interspecific social behaviour. Anim Behav. 84:1271–1277
Farris HE, Ryan MJ (2011) Relative comparisons of call parameters enable auditory grouping in frogs. Nat Commun 2:410
Farris HE, Ryan MJ (2017) Schema vs. primitive perceptual grouping: the relative weighting of sequential vs. spatial cues during an auditory grouping task in frogs. J Comp Physiol A 203:175–182
Fletcher RJ (2009) Does attraction to conspecifics explain the patch-size effect? An experimental test. Oikos 118:1139–1147
Friedl TW, Klump GM (2005) Sexual selection in the lek-breeding European treefrog: body size, chorus attendance, random mating and good genes. Anim Behav 70:1141–1154
Gerhardt HC, Huber F (2002) Acoustic communication in insects and anurans: Common problems and diverse solutions. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL
Gerhardt HC, Klump GM (1988) Masking of acoustic signals by the chorus background noise in the green tree frog: A limitation on mate choice. Anim Behav 36:1247–1249
Grafe T, Thein J (2001) Energetics of calling and metabolic substrate use during prolonged exercise in the European treefrog Hyla arborea. J Comp Physiol B 171:69–76
Halfwerk W, Lea AM, Guerra MA, Page RA, Ryan MJ (2016) Vocal responses to noise reveal the presence of the Lombard effect in a frog. Behav Ecol 27:669–676
Höbel G (2014) Socially mediated plasticity of chorusing behavior in the gladiator frog Hypsiboas rosenbergi. Acta Ethol 18:145–152
Höbel G, Barta T (2014) Adaptive plasticity in calling site selection in grey treefrogs (Hyla versicolor). Behaviour 151:741–754
Humfeld SC (2013) Condition-dependent signaling and adoption of mating tactics in an amphibian with energetic displays. Behav Ecol 24:859–870
Humfeld SC, Marshall VT, Bee MA (2009) Context-dependent plasticity of aggressive signalling in a dynamic social environment. Anim Behav 78:915–924
James MS, Stockwell MP, Clulow J, Clulow S, Mahony MJ (2015) Investigating behaviour for conservation goals: Conspecific call playback can be used to alter amphibian distributions within ponds. Biol Conserv 192:287–293
Kishi S, Nakazawa T (2013) Analysis of species coexistence co-mediated by resource competition and reproductive interference. Popul Ecol 55:305–313
Lu Y-T (2012) Study on resource partitioning of 6 frogs community in a still waterpond located in the tatung shan of New Taipei city. MSc thesis, National Taipei University of Education
Lucas JR, Howard RD, Palmer JG (1996) Callers and satellites: chorus behaviour in anurans as a stochastic dynamic game. Anim Behav 51:501–518
Luther D, Gentry K (2013) Sources of background noise and their influence on vertebrate acoustic communication. Behaviour 150:1045–1068
Madden N, Jehle R (2017) Acoustic orientation in the great crested newt (Triturus cristatus). Amphibia-Reptilia 38:57–65
Magrath RD, Haff TM, Fallow PM, Radford AN (2015) Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: from mechanisms to consequences. Biol Rev 90:560–586
Marshall VT, Schwartz JJ, Gerhardt HC (2006) Effects of heterospecific call overlap on the phonotactic behaviour of grey treefrogs. Anim Behav 72:449–459
Miller CW, Fletcher RJ Jr, Gillespie SR (2013) Conspecific and heterospecific cues override resource quality to influence offspring production. PLoS ONE 8:e70268
Mönkkönen M, Helle P, Niemi GJ, Montgomery K (1997) Heterospecific attraction affects community structure and migrant abundances in northern breeding bird communities. Can J Zool 75:2077–2083
Moreno-Gómez FN, Bacigalupe LD, Silva-Escobar AA, Soto-Gamboa M (2015) Female and male phonotactic responses and the potential effect of sexual selection on the advertisement calls of a frog. Anim Behav 104:79–86
Mou Y, Risch J, Lue K (1983) Rhacophorus prasinatus, a new tree frog from Taiwan, China (Amphibia, Anura, Rhacophoridae). Alytes 2:154–162
Mullet TC, Farina A, Gage SH (2017) The acoustic habitat hypothesis: an ecoacoustics perspective on species habitat selection. Biosemiotics 10:319–336.
Murphy CG (2003) The cause of correlations between nightly numbers of male and female barking treefrogs (Hyla gratiosa) attending choruses. Behav Ecol 14:274–281
Neelon DP, Höbel G (2017) Social plasticity in choosiness in green tree frogs, Hyla cinerea. Behav Ecol 28:1540–1546
Peig J, Green AJ (2010) The paradigm of body condition: a critical reappraisal of current methods based on mass and length. Funct Ecol 24:1323–1332
Phelps SM, Rand AS, Ryan MJ (2006) The mixed-species chorus as public information: tungara frogs eavesdrop on a heterospecific. Behav Ecol 18:108–114
Pizzatto L, Stockwell M, Clulow S, Clulow J, Mahony M (2015) Finding a place to live: conspecific attraction affects habitat selection in juvenile green and golden bell frogs. Acta Ethol 19:1–8
Prestwich KN (1994) The energetics of acoustic signaling in anurans and insects. Am Zool 34:625–643
Rehberg-Besler N, Doucet SM, Mennill DJ (2016) Overlapping vocalizations produce far-reaching choruses: a test of the signal enhancement hypothesis. Behav Ecol 28:494–499
Reichert MS, Gerhardt HC (2011) The role of body size on the outcome, escalation and duration of contests in the grey treefrog, Hyla versicolor. Anim Behav 82:1357–1366
Reichert MS, Symes LB, Höbel G (2016) Lighting up sound preferences: cross-modal influences on the precedence effect in treefrogs. Anim Behav 119:151–159
Rodríguez RL, Araya-Salas M, Gray DA, Reichert MS, Symes LB, Wilkins MR, Safran RJ, Höbel G (2014) How acoustic signals scale with individual body size: common trends across diverse taxa. Behav Ecol 26:168–177
Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD, Taft LK (1981) The costs and benefits of frog chorusing behavior. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 8:273–278
Schwartz JJ, Hunce R, Lentine B, Powers K (2015) Calling site choice and its impact on call degradation and call attractiveness in the gray treefrog, Hyla versicolor. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 70:1–19
Sebastian-Gonzalez E, Sanchez-Zapata JA, Botella F, Ovaskainen O (2010) Testing the heterospecific attraction hypothesis with time-series data on species co-occurrence. Proc R Soc Lond B 277:2983–2990
Stamps JA (1988) Conspecific attraction and aggregation in territorial species. Am Nat 131:329–347
Sullivan BK, Hinshaw SH (1992) Female choice and selection on male calling behavior in the gray treefrog Hyla versicolor. Anim Behav 44:733–744
Swanson EM, Tekmen SM, Bee MA (2007) Do female frogs exploit inadvertent social information to locate breeding aggregations? Can J Zool 85:921–932
Toledo LF, Martins IA, Bruschi DP, Passos MA, Alexandre C, Haddad CFB (2014) The anuran calling repertoire in the light of social context. Acta Ethol 18:87–99
Ursprung E, Hödl W, Ringler M (2009) Phonotactic approach pattern in the neotropical frog Allobates femoralis: A spatial and temporal analysis. Behaviour 146:153–170
Velez A, Bee MA (2010) Signal recognition by frogs in the presence of temporally fluctuating chorus-shaped noise. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:1695–1709
Wells KD (2001) The energetics of calling in frogs. In: Ryan MJ (ed) Anuran Communication. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC, pp 45–60
Wong S, Parada H, Narins PM (2009) Heterospecific acoustic interference: effects on calling in Oophaga pumilio. Biotropica 41:74–80
Zeigler SL, Neel MC, Oliveira L, Raboy BE, Fagan WF (2011) Conspecific and heterospecific attraction in assessments of functional connectivity. Biodivers Conserv 20:2779–2796
CH3
Amezquita A, Flechas SV, Lima AP, Gasser H, Hodl W (2011) Acoustic interference and recognition space within a complex assemblage of dendrobatid frogs. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:17058-63
Bee MA (2015) Treefrogs as animal models for research on auditory scene analysis and the cocktail party problem. Int J Psychophysiol 95:216-37
Bee MA, Schwartz JJ, Summers K (2013) All's well that begins Wells: celebrating 60 years of Anim Behav and 36 years of research on anuran social behaviour. Anim Behav 85:5-18
Bosch J, De la Riva I (2004) Are frog calls modulated by the environment? An analysis with anuran species from Bolivia. Can J Zool 82:880-888
Brepson L, Troïanowski M, Voituron Y, Lengagne T (2012a) Cheating for sex: inherent disadvantage or energetic constraint? Anim Behav 84:1253-1260
Brepson L, Voituron Y, Lengagne T (2012b) Condition-dependent ways to manage acoustic signals under energetic constraint in a tree frog. Behav Ecol 24:488-496
Capranica RR, Moffat AJ (1983) Neurobehavioral correlates of sound communication in anurans. In: Advances in vertebrate neuroethology. Springer Press, New York
Clark CJ (2012) The role of power versus energy in courtship: what is the ‘energetic cost’ of a courtship display? Anim Behav 84:269-277
Chen S-L (1992) The reproductive behavior and ecology of emerald green tree frog (Rhacophorus smaragdinus). MSc thesis, National Taiwan Normal University
Cui J, Song X, Zhu B, Fang G, Tang Y, Ryan MJ (2016) Receiver discriminability drives the evolution of complex sexual signals by sexual selection. Evolution 70:922-7
Cunnington GM, Fahrig L (2013) Mate attraction by male anurans in the presence of traffic noise. Anim Conserv 16:275-285
Erdtmann LK, Lima AP (2013) Environmental effects on anuran call design: what we know and what we need to know. Ethol Ecol Evol 25:1-11
Gall MD, Wilczynski W (2016) The effects of call-like masking diminish after nightly exposure to conspecific choruses in green treefrogs (Hyla cinerea). J Exp Biol 219:1295-302
Garcia-Rutledge EJ, Narins PM (2001) Shared acoustic resources in an old world frog community. Herpetologica:104-116
Goutte, Argo, Argo IV, Kime (2010) Calling strategies of male túngara frogs in response to dynamic playback. Behaviour 147:65-83
Goutte S, Dubois A, Howard SD, Marquez R, Rowley JJ, Dehling JM, Grandcolas P, Rongchuan X, Legendre F (2016) Environmental constraints and call evolution in torrent-dwelling frogs. Evolution 70:811-26
Gregory Byrne P (2008) Strategic male calling behavior in an australian terrestrial toadlet (Pseudophryne bibronii). Copeia 2008:57-63
Höbel G, Barta T (2014) Adaptive plasticity in calling site selection in grey treefrogs (Hyla versicolor). Behaviour 151:741-754
Höbel G, Gerhardt HC (2007) Sources of selection on signal timing in a tree frog. Ethology 113:973-982
Hobel G (2014) Effect of temporal and spectral noise features on gap detection behavior by calling green treefrogs. Behav Processes 108:43-9
Humfeld SC (2013) Condition-dependent signaling and adoption of mating tactics in an amphibian with energetic displays. Behav Ecol 24:859-870
Judge KA, Brooks RJ (2001) Chorus participation by male bullfrogs, Rana catesbeiana: a test of the energetic constraint hypothesis. Anim Behav 62:849-861
Kaiser K, Hammers J (2009) The effect of anthropogenic noise on male advertisement call rate in the neotropical treefrog, Dendropsophus triangulum. Behaviour 146:1053-1069
Kaiser K, Scofield DG, Alloush M, Jones RM, Marczak S, Martineau K, Oliva MA, Narins PM (2011) When sounds collide: the effect of anthropogenic noise on a breeding assemblage of frogs in Belize, Central America. Behaviour 148:215-232
Leary CJ, Lippincott J, Harris S, Hawkins DL (2015) A test of the Energetics-Hormone Vocalization model in the green treefrog. Gen Comp Endocrinol 213:32-9
Lee N, Ward JL, Velez A, Micheyl C, Bee MA (2017) Frogs exploit statistical regularities in noisy acoustic scenes to solve cocktail-party-like problems. Curr Biol 27:743-750
Llusia D, Gomez M, Penna M, Marquez R (2013) Call transmission efficiency in native and invasive anurans: competing hypotheses of divergence in acoustic signals. PLoS One 8:e77312
Luther D, Magnotti J (2014) Can animals detect differences in vocalizations adjusted for anthropogenic noise? Anim Behav 92:111-116
Mou Y, Risch J, Lue K (1983) Rhacophorus prasinatus, a new tree frog from Taiwan, China (Amphibia, Anura, Rhacophoridae). Alytes 2:154-162
Mullet TC, Farina A, Gage SH (2017) The acoustic habitat hypothesis: an ecoacoustics perspective on species habitat selection. Biosemiotics 10:319-336
Murphy CG (1994) Determinants of chorus tenure in barking treefrogs (Hyla gratiosa). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 34:285-294
Paez VP, Bock BC, Rand AS (1993) Inhibition of evoked calling of Dendrobates pumilio due to acoustic interference from cicada calling. Biotropica 25:242-245
Peig J, Green AJ (2010) The paradigm of body condition: a critical reappraisal of current methods based on mass and length. Funct Ecol 24:1323-1332
Rand AS (1985) Tradeoffs in the evolution of frog calls. Proceedings: Animal Sciences 94:623-637
Roca IT, Desrochers L, Giacomazzo M, Bertolo A, Bolduc P, Deschesnes R, Martin CA, Rainville V, Rheault G, Proulx R (2016) Shifting song frequencies in response to anthropogenic noise: a meta-analysis on birds and anurans. Behav Ecol 27:1269-1274
Ruppe L, Clement G, Herrel A, Ballesta L, Decamps T, Kever L, Parmentier E (2015) Environmental constraints drive the partitioning of the soundscape in fishes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:6092-7
Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD, Taft LK (1981) The costs and benefits of frog chorusing behavior. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 8:273-278
Schrode KM, Bee MA (2015) Evolutionary adaptations for the temporal processing of natural sounds by the anuran peripheral auditory system. J Exp Biol 218:837-48
Schwartz JJ (1993) Male calling behavior, female discrimination and acoustic interference in the Neotropical treefrog Hyla microcephala under realistic acoustic conditions. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 32:401-414
Sheehan KL, Esswein ST, Dorr BS, Yarrow GK, Johnson RJ (2017) Using species distribution models to define nesting habitat of the eastern metapopulation of double‐crested cormorants. Ecol Evol 7:409-418
Stuckert AM, Saporito RA, Summers K (2018). An empirical test indicates only qualitatively honest aposematic signaling within a population of vertebrates. J Herpetol 52:201-208
Tárano Z, Hebets E (2015) Choosing a mate in a cocktail party-like situation: the effect of call complexity and call timing between two rival males on female Mating preferences in the Túngara frog Physalaemus pustulosus. Ethology 121:749-759
Tarano Z, Carballo L (2016) Call intercalation in dyadic interactions in natural choruses of Johnstone's whistling frog Eleutherodactylus johnstonei (Anura: Eleutherodactylidae). Behav Processes 126:55-63
Vélez A, Schwartz JJ, Bee MA (2013) Anuran signal perception in noisy environments. In: Acoustic Communication in Noise (Ed. Brumm H.) Springer Handbook of Animal Signals and Communication. Springer Press, New York
Vargas-Salinas F, Amézquita A (2013) Traffic noise correlates with calling time but not spatial distribution in the threatened poison frog Andinobates bombetes. Behaviour 150:569-584
Villanueva-Rivera LJ (2014) Eleutherodactylus frogs show frequency but no temporal partitioning: implications for the acoustic niche hypothesis. PeerJ 2:e496
Voituron Y, Richardson C, Joly P, Lengagne T, Brepson L (2012) Energetics of calling in the male treefrog Hyla arborea: when being large means being sexy at low cost. Behaviour 149:775-793
Wells KD (2010) The ecology and behavior of amphibians. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
CH4
Ahlering MA, Arlt D, Betts MG, Fletcher Jr RJ, Nocera JJ, Ward MP (2010) Research needs and recommendations for the use of conspecific-attraction methods in the conservation of migratory songbirds. The Condor 112:252-264
Anderson RB (2019) Human traffic and habitat complexity are strong predictors for the distribution of a declining amphibian. PloS one 14:e0213426
Andersson M, Simmons LW (2006) Sexual selection and mate choice. Trends Ecol Evol 21:296-302
Bee MA, Schwartz JJ, Summers K (2013) All's well that begins Wells: celebrating 60 years of Anim Behav and 36 years of research on anuran social behaviour. Anim Behav 85:5-18
Beebee TJ, Griffiths RA (2005) The amphibian decline crisis: a watershed for conservation biology? Bio Conserv 125:271-285
Bosch J, De la Riva I (2004) Are frog calls modulated by the environment? An analysis with anuran species from Bolivia. Can J Zool 82:880-888
Brepson L, Voituron Y, Lengagne T (2012) Condition-dependent ways to manage acoustic signals under energetic constraint in a tree frog. Behav Ecol 24:488-496
Buxton VL, Ward MP, Sperry JH, Foster S (2017) Frog breeding pond selection in response to predators and conspecific cues. Ethology 123:397-404
Buxton VL, Ward MP, Sperry JH (2018) Evaluation of conspecific attraction as a management tool across several species of Anurans. Diversity 10:6
Cunningham HR, Rissler LJ, Buckley LB, Urban MC (2016) Abiotic and biotic constraints across reptile and amphibian ranges. Ecography 39:1-8
Cushman SA (2006) Effects of habitat loss and fragmentation on amphibians: a review and prospectus. Biol Conser 128:231-240
Dawkins R, Krebs JR (1979) Arms races between and within species. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 205:489-511
Friedl TWP, Klump GM (2005) Sexual selection in the lek-breeding European treefrog: body size, chorus attendance, random mating and good genes. Anim Behav 70:1141-1154
Gerhardt HC, Huber F (2002) Acoustic communication in insects and anurans: common problems and diverse solutions. University of Chicago Press
Goutte S, Dubois A, Howard SD, Marquez R, Rowley JJ, Dehling JM, Grandcolas P, Rongchuan X, Legendre F (2016) Environmental constraints and call evolution in torrent-dwelling frogs. Evolution 70:811-26
Halfwerk W, Blaas M, Kramer L, Hijner N, Trillo PA, Bernal XE, Page RA, Goutte S, Ryan MJ, Ellers J (2019) Adaptive changes in sexual signalling in response to urbanization. Nat Ecol Evol 3:374
Henry CS, Wells MM (2010) Acoustic niche partitioning in two cryptic sibling species of Chrysoperla green lacewings that must duet before mating. Anim Behav 80:991-1003
Humfeld SC (2013) Condition-dependent signaling and adoption of mating tactics in an amphibian with energetic displays. Behav Ecol 24:859-870
Kaiser K, Scofield DG, Alloush M, Jones RM, Marczak S, Martineau K, Oliva MA, Narins PM (2011) When sounds collide: the effect of anthropogenic noise on a breeding assemblage of frogs in Belize, Central America. Behaviour 148:215-232
Kishi S, Nakazawa T (2013) Analysis of species coexistence co-mediated by resource competition and reproductive interference. Popul Ecol 55:305-313
Muller BJ, Schwarzkopf L (2017) Relative effectiveness of trapping and hand-capture for controlling invasive cane toads (Rhinella marina). Int J Pest Manag 64:158-192
Mullet TC, Farina A, Gage SH (2017) The Acoustic Habitat Hypothesis: An Ecoacoustics Perspective on Species Habitat Selection. Biosemiotics 10:319-336
Nishiumi N, Mori A (2016) Immobile defence of a frog distracts attention of approaching predators to other prey. Behaviour 153:1387-1401
Rand AS (1985) Tradeoffs in the evolution of frog calls. Proc Anim Sci 94:623-637
Roca IT, Desrochers L, Giacomazzo M, Bertolo A, Bolduc P, Deschesnes R, Martin CA, Rainville V, Rheault G, Proulx R (2016) Shifting song frequencies in response to anthropogenic noise: a meta-analysis on birds and anurans. Behav Ecol 27:1269-1274
Ruppe L, Clement G, Herrel A, Ballesta L, Decamps T, Kever L, Parmentier E (2015) Environmental constraints drive the partitioning of the soundscape in fishes. PNAS 112:6092-7
Schwarzkopf L, Alford R (2007) Acoustic attractants enhance trapping success for cane toads. Wildl Res 34:366-370
Tingley R, Ward-Fear G, Schwarzkopf L, Greenlees MJ, Phillips BL, Brown G, Clulow S, Webb J, Capon R, Sheppard A (2017) New weapons in the Toad Toolkit: a review of methods to control and mitigate the biodiversity impacts of invasive cane toads (Rhinella marina). Q Rev Biol 92:123-149
Vélez A, Schwartz JJ, Bee MA (2013) Anuran signal perception in noisy environments. In: Acoustic Communication in Noise (Ed. Brumm H.) Springer Handbook of Animal Signals and Communication. Springer Press, New York
Ward MP, Schlossberg S (2004) Conspecific attraction and the conservation of territorial songbirds. Conserv Biol 18:519-525