簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 黃嘉笙
Huang Chia Sheng
論文名稱: 立高與知覺系統引致之登階高度環境賦使
Affordances for stair climbing induced by standing heights and perceptual systems
指導教授: 楊梓楣
Yang, Chih-Mei
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 體育學系
Department of Physical Education
論文出版年: 2009
畢業學年度: 97
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 70
中文關鍵詞: 環境賦使觸動覺視覺最大登階高度
英文關鍵詞: affordances, haptic perception, visual perception, maximum riser height
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:222下載:14
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 環境賦使乃是生態觀點中描繪動物與環境間交織的一種關係,環境特性的定義必須參照動物本身的條件,其透過身體尺度的概念來詮釋。本研究基於生態觀點旨在探討觸動覺與視覺導引之登階動作可能性,並藉由個體站立高度的改變,檢視其知覺最大登階高度之環境賦使。24 名成人參與本實驗,並隨機分派為觸動覺與視覺評估兩組,參與者被要求操作手杖進行探索活動,與直接透過視覺觀察兩種方式,評估能否以單腳抬起之動作攀登台階。實驗安排參與者站立於真實地板和高 10cm 之升高地板,以及穿著高 10cm 的積木鞋共三種情境。運用可調整高度之台階擷取參與者的知覺最大登階高度,並依據腿長計算登階高度之關鍵比值。此外,採用 2(知覺系統) × 3(立高)二因子混合設計變異數分析考驗知覺評估的恆常誤差、絕對誤差、信心分數、與評估時間。根據結果與討論發現:最大登階高度之環境賦使並不會受到個體站立高度改變而影響其知覺結果,個體腿長與台階結構的關係會維持不變的特質。以觸動覺探索的知覺最大登階高度,可採用和視覺觀察相同的身體尺度概念來描述,不管站立高度如何改變,其關鍵比值約為 0.97。儘管個體透過不同知覺系統覺知最大登階高度,其準確性與信心都相當一致,支持知覺系統對等的觀點。

    Affordances in ecological psychology viewpoint mean a kind of interlaced relation between the animals and its environment. The definition for the properties of environment should refer to the characteristics of animals and be interpreted through the concept of body scale. This study was based on the ecological approach to investigate haptically and visually guided stair climbing possibility. We examined affordances for perceived maximum riser heights by changing individual’s standing heights. 24 adults were recruited in this experiment, and randomly assigned to two groups, the haptic judgement group and the visual judgement group. The participants were asked to judge whether each stair height is climbable if they only raised one single leg. The judgments were made through two means: Visual observation and exploration with a white cane. Three experimental condition were arranged when participants made their judgments: Standing on the real floor, standing on 10cm height raising floor, and wear 10cm height blocks. The adjustable stair was used to measure perceived maximum raiser heights of participants, and the critical ratio was calculated by comparing it to the leg length of participants. Furthermore, 2 (perceptual systems) ×3 (standing heights) mixed design two-way ANOVAs were used to examine the constant errors of perceptual judgment, the absolute errors of perceptual judgment, the scores of confidence, and the time of judgment. Based on the results we found that the perception of affordances for maximum riser heights would not be influenced by standing heights. The relation between the lengths of the individual’s legs and the stair heights would maintain invariant. The perceived maximum raisers height by the haptic exploration could also be described by the concept of body scale with the visual observation. The critical ratio was approximately 0.97, no matter what the standing heights were. Despite individuals perceived maximum raiser heights with different perceptual systems, the accuracy and confidence of haptic and visual judgment did not differ. It supported the viewpoint of perceptual equivalence.

    第一章 緒論......................................1 第一節 問題背景.................................. 1 第二節 研究問題與假說.............................11 第三節 研究基本假定與限制..........................12 第四節 名詞解釋...................................12 第五節 研究重要性.................................14 第二章 文獻探討….................................15 第一節 身體尺度...................................15 第二節 動物-環境的動態適配.........................22 第三節 觸動覺系統…................................25 第四節 文獻小節....................................32 第三章 方法........................................34 第一節 實驗參與者與實驗工作..........................34 第二節 實驗設備與場地佈置…..........................35 第三節 實驗流程.....................................39 第四節 資料處理與統計分析............................41 第四章 結果........................................45 第一節 知覺最大登階高度與評估準確性...................45 第二節 參照身體尺度之登階高度關鍵比值.................51 第三節 信心評分與評估時間............................52 第五章 討論........................................54 第一節 站立高度對知覺登階環境賦使之效應...............54 第二節 以行動為基礎的知覺系統對等.....................57 第六章 結論與建議...................................60 第一節 結論.........................................60 第二節 建議.........................................61 引用文獻..............................................62 附錄..................................................65 附錄一 參與者須知及同意書.............................65 附錄二 慣用手問卷....................................66 附錄三 實驗流程圖....................................67 附錄四 參與者基本資料.................................68 附錄五 變異數分析摘要表...............................69

    引用文獻
    中文部份
    楊玉芳(2002)。第五章視覺與聽覺。載於朱瀅編:實驗心理學(173-229 頁)。台北市:五南。

    瞿錦春、張芬芬(譯)(2006)。視覺心理學。臺北市:五南。(Gregory, R. L., 1997)

    英文部份
    Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology: Concepts and methods for studying the environment of human behavior. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Burton, A. W., & Davis, W. E. (1996). Ecological task analysis utilizing intrinsic measures in research and practice. Human Movement Science, 15(2), 285-314.

    Burton, G. (1992). Nonvisual judgment of the crossability of path gaps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 698-713.

    Carello, C. (2004). Perceiving affordance by dynamic touch: Hint from the control of movement. Ecological Psychology, 16(1), 31-36.

    Carello. C., Thuot, S., Anderson, K. L., & Turvey, M, T. (1999). Perceiving the sweet spot. Perception, 28(3), 307-320.

    Carello, C., Thuot, S., & Turvey, M. T. (2000). Aging and the perception of a racket's sweet spot. Human Movement Science, 19(1),1-20.

    Chang, C. H., Wade, M. G., Stoffregen, T. A., & Ho, H. Y. (2008). Length perception by dynamic touch: The effects of aging and experience. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 63B(3), 165-170.

    Fitzpatrick, P., Carello, C., Schmdit, R. C., & Corey, D. (1994). Haptic and visual perception of an affordance for upright posture. Ecological Psychology, 6(4), 265-287.

    Gibson, J. J. (1961). Ecological optical. Vision Research, 1(3-4), 253-262.

    Gibson, J. J. (1966). The sense considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associate. (Original work published 1979)

    Hirose, N., & Nishio, A. (2001). The process of adaptation to perceiving new action capabilities. Ecological Psychology, 13(1), 49-69.

    Hove, P., Riley, M. A., & Shockley, K. (2006). Perceiving affordaces of hockey sticks by dynamic touch. Ecological Psychology, 18(3), 163-189.

    Ingle, D., & Cook, J. (1977). The effects of viewing distance upon size preference of frog for prey. Vision Research, 17(9), 1009-1019.

    Klevberg, G. L., & Anderson, D. I. (2002). Visual and haptic perception of postural affordances in children and adults. Human Movement Science, 21(2), 169-186.

    Köhler, W. (1976). The mentality of apes. (E. Winter, Trans.). New York: Liveright. (Original work published 1925)

    Kreifeldt, J. G., & Chuang, M. C. (1979). Moment of inertia: Psychphysical study of an overlooked sensation. Science, 206(2), 588-590.

    Mark, L. S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 361-370.

    Mark, L. S., Balliet, J. A., Craver, K. D., Douglas, S. D., & Fox, T. (1990). What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for setting. Ecological Psychology, 2(4), 325-366.

    Mark, L. S., & Vogele, D. (1987). A biodynamic basis for perceived categories of action: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19(3), 367-384.

    Michaels, C. F., & Carello, C. (1981). Direct perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F. C., & Dolné, M. A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordance: Perception of catchableness fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(4), 879-891.

    Pagano, C. C., & Turvey, M. T. (1998). Eigenvectors of the inertia tensor and perceiving the orientations of limbs and objects. Journal of Applied Biomechanics, 14, 331-359.

    Regia-Corte, T., & Wagman, J. B. (2008). Perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface depends on height of center of mass. Experimental Brain Research, 191(1), 25-35.

    Solomon, H. Y., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Haptically perceiving the distance reachable with hand-held objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 404-427.

    Stoffregen, T. A., Yang, C. M., & Bardy, B. G. (2005). Affordance judgments and nonlocomotor body movement. Ecological Psychology, 17(2), 75-104.

    Turvey, M. T. (1996). Dynamic touch. American Psychologist, 51(11), 1134-1152.

    Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2001). Affordance and inertial constraints on tool use. Ecological Psychology, 13(3), 173-195.

    Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2003). Haptically creating affordence: The user-tool interface. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9(3), 175-186.

    Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 683-703.

    Warren, W. H., & Whang, S. (1987). Visual guidance of walking through apertures: Body-scaled information for affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 371-383.

    下載圖示
    QR CODE