研究生: |
莊庭瑜 Chuang, Ting-Yu |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
奮力控制方案對幼兒專注力提升效果的探討 The Effects of Effortful Control Program on Preschoolers’ Attention |
指導教授: |
鍾志從
Jong, Jyh-Tsorng |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
人類發展與家庭學系 Department of Human Development and Family Studies |
論文出版年: | 2021 |
畢業學年度: | 109 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 123 |
中文關鍵詞: | 幼兒 、準實驗研究 、專注力 、奮力控制 |
英文關鍵詞: | preschoolers, quasi-experimental reseacch design, attention, effortful control |
DOI URL: | http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202100212 |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:324 下載:0 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
本研究旨在發展適合幼兒的奮力控制方案,瞭解幼兒專注力目前水準,探討奮力控制方案對幼兒專注力提升的成效,以及個人變項不同幼兒專注力的差異。
以新北市兩所非營利幼兒園為取樣對象,其中一所幼兒園的兩個班級為實驗組,而另一所幼兒園的兩個班級為對照組,排除一位中途退出研究的幼兒,共61位幼兒完成前測、後測和後後測(實驗組31位,對照組30位)。實驗階段,實驗組幼兒參與為期八週,共三十二次的方案介入,對照組幼兒則沒有參與任何實驗。研究工具採用NeuroSky 腦波檢測儀蒐集幼兒專注力資料。在奮力控制方案介入的前、後一週對實驗組與對照組進行前測與後測,並於實驗結束後四週對實驗組和對照組進行後後測。透過SPSS23.0的統計分析,本研究的結果如下:
一、奮力控制方案的實驗介入後,與對照組比較,實驗組幼兒的專注力並無
顯著的立即改變效果。
二、奮力控制方案的實驗介入,實驗後四週與對照組比較,實驗組幼兒的專
注力具有顯著的保留效果。
三、奮力控制方案的實驗介入,對實驗組幼兒而言,僅有low beta波有顯
著的改變,但在奮力控制方案結束四週後實驗組幼兒的專注力數值high
alpha、low beta和high beta均顯著優於活動介入前。
四、奮力控制方案的實驗介入,性別不同,實驗組幼兒在後後測的
highbeta具有顯著的差異。女生改變較明顯,平均值低於男生。
五、奮力控制方案的實驗介入,年齡不同,實驗組幼兒的專注力表現在後測
與後後測均未有顯著差異。
六、奮力控制的實驗介入,家庭社經地位不同,實驗組幼兒在後測的腦波有
顯著的改變。在low alpha中,高家庭社經地位幼兒的專注力數值改變
較明顯,平均低於中家庭社經地位;中家庭社經地位幼兒low beta波
的專注力數值改變較明顯,平均低於低家庭社經地位的幼兒。
Developing adequate methods of the effortful control program, the purposes of this study were to understand the average level of preschoolers’ attention, discuss the improving outcomes of the effortful control program on preschoolers’ attention, and examine the differences of gender, age, SES on the preschoolers’ attention.
The subjects came from four classes of the two non-profit preschools in New Taipei City Taiwan. Two of them were in the experimental group, and the other two classes were in the control group. Except for one kid who dropped out halfway through the experiment, a total of sixty-one preschoolers participated in this study. The survey included pretest, posttest, and post-posttest. Pretests were conducted one week before the effort control program joined. Posttests happened one week after the intervention. Post-posttests were sent out four weeks after the effort control program joined. With the intervention of effortful control program, thirty-one preschoolers in the experimental group had thrity-two activities taught by the researcher. The activities included effortful control elements were conducted four to five times a week for eight consecutive weeks. Thirty preschoolers in the control group had not requested any test.
The NeuroSky brainwave starter was used to collect preschoolers’ attention data. Through the analysis of SPSS 23.0, five results of this study were summarized as follows:
1.Compared with the control group, there was no significant change to the attention level sign of the experimental group after the effortful control was inserted.
2.Compared with the control group, there was a significant retention effect to the attention level signs of the experimental group after four weeks the effortful control was intervened.
3.There was a significant difference to the low beta wave of the experimental group after effortful control was intervened. However, , the attention level signs of high alpha wave, low beta wave, and high beta wave in post-posttest were significantly lower than the signs in the pretest.
4.After effortful control was intervened, there was a significant difference in the high beta signs of post- posttest between gender in the experimental group. That is, the average high beta signs of the girls were lower than the boy’s high beta signs.
5.When the preschooler’s age was different, there were no difference to the attention signs performance were between posttest and post-posttest.
6.After effortful control was intervened, there was a significant difference in the low alpha and low beta signs of posttest between family and social economic statuses in the experimental group. That is, the average low alpha signs of the high family and social economic statuses were
lower than the signs of the middle family and social economic statuses. The average low beta signs of the middle family and social economic statuses were lower than the signs of the low family and social economic statuses.
參考文獻
壹、中文文獻
王玲和孫敬哲(2016)。藝術治療教學方案對國小輕度障礙學生提昇專注力之研究。雲嘉特教期刊,23,50-57。
朱慶琳、黃敏怡、葉啟斌和姜忠信(2018)。學齡期注意力不足過動症—混合表現型兒童執行功能研究。特殊教育研究學刊,43(1),53-78。
吳盈諄(2013)。幼兒執行功能與就學準備度之研究(未出版碩士論文)。國立政治大學。
宋淑慧(1992)。多向度注意力測驗編製之研究(未出版碩士論文)。國立彰化師範大學。
李咏庭、王加恩、王巧涵和李盈潔(2017)。ADHD 兒童執行功能訓練團體之成效。諮商與輔導,384,22-25。
汪欣怡和鍾志從(2018)。靜聽音樂對幼兒專注力提昇之實驗研究。輔仁民生學誌,24(1),81-98。
阮啟弘、呂岱樺、劉佳蓉和陳巧雲(2005)。視覺注意力的研究議題與神經生理機制。應用心理研究,28,25-50。
周台傑、邱上真和宋淑慧(1993)。多向度注意力測驗。心理。
周昱君(2014)。家庭社經地位、教育資源對幼兒認知能力表現的影響:學前教育機會均等的檢視(未出版碩士論文)。國立東華大學。
周恩存(2012)。「讀你的腦」-腦波量測新技術在人類認知、學習及生活上的應用。數位典藏與學習電子報,11(11)。取自http://newsletter.teldap.tw/news/NewsContent.php?nid=6121&lid=706
周靜靜、汪保華和呂志華(2011)。腦電圖α波與運動研究進展。湖北醫藥學院學報,30(1),99-1-1。
林玉雯、黃台珠和劉嘉茹(2010)。課室學習專注力之研究-量表發展與分析應用。科學教育學刊,18(2),107-129。
林宜親、李冠慧、宋玟欣、柯華葳、曾志朗、洪蘭和阮啟弘(2011)。以認知神經科學取向探討兒童注意力的發展和學習之關聯。教育心理學報,42(3),517-542。
林儀婷(2006)。自閉症幼兒相互注意力介入研究—遊戲取向(未出版碩士論文)。國立臺北教育大學。
林鋐宇、李柏森、張文典和洪福源(2013)。不同性別注意力缺陷過動 症與普通學童之注意力表現差異探究。特殊教育研究學刊,3(82),29-51 。
林鋐宇和周台傑(2010)。國小兒童注意力測驗之編製。特殊教育研究學刊,35(2),29-53。
張玉蓉(2017)。注意力不足/過動症高危險群兒童的主動控制、父母教養方式與兒童問題行為之關係探討 (未出版碩士論文)。東吳大學。
張春興(2013)。教育心理學-三化取向的理論與實踐重修二版。東華。
張媛媛(2003)。藝術治療對國小學習障礙兒童提昇注意力的輔導效果(未出版碩士論文)臺中師範學院。
張鳳吟(2006)。學前幼兒情緒性,情緒調節與同儕關係之研究 (未出版碩士論文)。 國立臺南大學。
陳君如(2003)。國小學生注意力訓練方案之實驗研究(未出版碩士論文)。臺北市立師範學院。
陳雅鈴(2015)。發展經濟弱勢幼兒執行功能課程之問題及成效。教育心理學報,47(2),261-279。
陳資韻(2008)。新聞版面配置對投資人注意力影響之研究(未出版碩士論文)。國立雲林科技大學。
黃慧娟(2013)。融入式音樂活動教學於幼兒專注力訓練課程之探究。幼兒保育學刊,10,137-149。
黃毅志(2003)。臺灣地區新職業聲望與社經地位量表之建構與評估: 社會科學與教育社會學研究本土化。教育研究集刊,49(4),1-31。
葉芳瑜(2013)。家長食物選擇與幼兒飲料零食攝取及幼兒情緒調節之關係(未出版碩士論文)。朝陽科技大學。
劉百純和鍾志從(2016)。幼兒專注力提昇方案之實驗研究-以幼兒園按摩活動為例。長庚科技學刊,25,119-136。
劉長宗和饒見維(2015)。正念靜坐對提昇國小學童專注力之研究。佛學與科學,16(2),81-98。
蕭夙婷和張瓊云(2009)。從神經生理觀點探討幼兒注意力之表現。幼兒保育論壇,4,8-26。
蕭愛霖和鍾志從(2016年5月20日)。圍棋活動對幼兒空間能力及專注力的影響[海報發表]。輔仁大學兒童與家庭學系「兒家• In •社區」學術研討會,新北市,臺灣。
貳、外文文獻
Agostino, A., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2010). Executive functions underlying multiplicative reasoning: Problem type matters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(4), 286-305.
Babiloni C., Del Percio C., Iacoboni M., Infarinato F., Lizio R., Marzano N., Crespi G., Dassù F., Pirritano M., Gallamini M., & Eusebi F. (2008) Golf putt outcomes are predicted by sensorimotor cerebral EEG rhythms. Journal of Physiology,586 (1), 131-139.
Belle, A., Hargraves, R. H., & Najarian, K. (2012). An automated optimal engagement and attention detection system using electrocardiogram. Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine, 2012, 1-12. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/528781.
Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29(3), 180-200.
Bierman, K. L., Nix, R. L., Greenberg, M. T., Blair, C., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2008). Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 821-843.
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111-127.
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2001). Tools of the mind: A case study of implementing the Vygotskian approach in American early childhood and primary classrooms. Geneva, International Bureau of Education, UNESCO.
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624.
Brodeur, D. A., & Pond, M. (2001). The development of selective attention in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(3), 229-239.
Busch, B., Biederman, J., Cohen, L. G., Sayer, J. M., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Zallen, B., & Faraone, S. V. (2002). Correlates of ADHD among children in pediatric and psychiatric clinics. Psychiatric Services, 53(9), 1103-1111.
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215-222.
Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Kim, J. S.,Voss, M. W., & Vanpatter, M.(2010). A neuroimaging investigation of the association between aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume, and memory performance in preadolescent children. Brain Research, 1358, 172-183.
Clark, K. E., & Ladd, G. W. (2000). Connectedness and autonomy support in parent–child relationships: Links to children's socioemotional orientation and peer relationships. Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 485.
Clark, R. C., Nguyen, F, & Sweller, J. (2006). Efficiency in learning: Evidence-based guidelines to manage cognitive load. Pfeiffer.
Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., McDowell, J. E., Austin, B. P., Miller, P. H., Yanasak, N. E., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: A randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychology, 30, 91-98.
Davis, E. P., Bruce, J., & Gunnar, M. R. (2002). The anterior attention network: Associations with temperament and neuroendocrine activity in 6‐year‐old children. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 40(1), 43-56.
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4), 633-652.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168.
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387-1388.
Duchowski, A. T. (2003). Eye tracking methodology: Theory and practice. Springer-Verlag.
Dumont, T. Q. (2006). The power of concentration. Retrieved November 12, 2007, from http://www.jogena.com/resources/concentration.pdf.
Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., Sadovsky, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Effortful control. In R.F. Baumeister & K.D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 259–282). The Guilford Press.
Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T. L., Cumberland, A., Liew, J., Reiser, M., Zhou, Q. & Losoya, S. H. (2009). Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 988.
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children's effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three‐wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76(5), 1055-1071.
Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Gender differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 33-72.
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143-149.
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143-149.
Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for adult temperament. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(4), 868-888.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I., & Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional networks. NeuroImage, 26, 471-479.
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2002). Early intervention and the development of self-regulation. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 22(3), 307-335.
Fossella, J., Sommer T., Fan, J., Wu, Y., Swanson, J. M., Pfaff, D. W., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Assessing the molecular genetics of attention networks. BMC Neuroscience, 3, 14.
Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Fan, J., Wu, Y., Swanson, J. M., Pfaff, D. W., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Assessing the molecular genetics of attention networks. BMC Neuroscience, 3(1), 14.
Fox, J. R., Park, B., & Lang, A. (2007). When available resources become negative resources: The effects of cognitive overload on memory sensitivity and criterion bias. Communication Research, 34(3), 277-296.
Frick, M. A., & Brocki, K. C. (2019). A multi-factorial perspective on ADHD and ODD in school-aged children: What is the role of cognitive regulation, temperament, and parental support?. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 41(9), 933-945.
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Robinson, J. L., & Hewitt, J. K. (2011). Developmental trajectories in toddlers' self-restraint predict individual differences in executive functions 14 years later: A behavioral genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1410.
Gagné, R. M. (1985). The conditions of learning and theory of instruction (4th Ed.). Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31.
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 18(1), 1-16.
Grünewald-Zuberbier, E., Grünewald, G., & Rasche, A. (1975). Hyperactive behavior and EEG arousal reactions children. in Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 38(2), 149-159.
Harman, C., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (1997). Distress and attention interactions in early infancy. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 27-43
Harvey, W. J., & Reid, G. (2003). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review of research on movement skill performance and physical fitness. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 20(1), 1-25.
Hinshaw, S. P. (2018). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Controversy, developmental mechanisms, and multiple levels of analysis. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 14(1), 291-316.
Hughes, M. A., Alberto, P. A., & Fredrick, L. L. (2006). Self-operated auditory prompting systems as a function-based intervention in public community settings. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 8(4), 230-243.
Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M. B., O'Leary, K. C., Scudder, M. R., Wu, C., Castelli, D. M., & Hillman, C. H. (2011). The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in preadolescent children. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1046–1058.
Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2012). Effortful control, depression, and anxiety correlate with the influence of emotion on executive attentional control. Biological Psychology, 91(1), 88-95.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67(2), 490-507.
Krieger, V., Amador-Campos, J. A., & Gallardo-Pujol, D. (2019). Temperament, executive function, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adolescents: The mediating role of effortful control. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 41(6), 615–633.
Larsen, E. A. (2011). Classification of eeg signals in a brain-computer interface system (Unpublished master’s thesis).Norwegian University of Science and Technology.
Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused : Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 75-82.
Li, X., Hu, B., Zhu, T., Yan, J., & Zheng, F. (2009). Towards affective learning with an EEG feedback approach. In Proceedings of the first ACM international workshop on multimedia technologies for distance learning (pp. 33-38). ACM.
Martel, M. M., & Nigg, J. T. (2006). Child ADHD and personality/temperament traits of reactive and effortful control, resiliency, and emotionality. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(11), 1175-1183.
McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2006). The impact of kindergarten learning-related skills on academic trajectories at the end of elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21(4), 471-490.
Mecklinger, A., Kramer, A. F., & Strayer, D. L. (1992). Event related potentials and EEG components in a semantic memory search task. Psychophysiology, 29(1), 104-119.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.
Molander, B., & Bäckman, L. (1989). Age differences in heart rate patterns during concentration in a precision sport: Implications for attentional functioning. Journal of Gerontology, 44(3), 80-87.
Murugappan, M., Nagarajan, R., & Yaacob, S. ( 2009 ). Comparison of different wavelet features from EEG signals for classifying human emotions. IEEE Symposium on Industrial Electronics & Applications, Kuala Lumpur, 836-841.
Nigg, J. T., Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2005). Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1224–1230.
Noble, K. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Socioeconomic background modulates cognition–achievement relationships in reading. Cognitive Development, 21(3), 349-368.
Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8(1), 74-87.
Onton, J., Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2005). Frontal midline EEG dynamics during working memory. Neuroimage, 27(2), 341-356.
Palva, S., & Palva, J. M. (2007). New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(4), 150-158.
Pfurtscheller, G., & Klimesch, W. (1992). Event-related synchronization and desynchronization of alpha and beta waves in a cognitive task. In Induced rhythms in the brain (pp. 117-128). Birkhäuser.
Poltavski, D. V., Biberdorf, D., & Petros, T. V. (2012). Accommodative response and cortical activity during sustained attention. Vision Research, 63, 1-8.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3-25.
Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. Nuclear Medicine, 28(4), 278-289.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Attention, self–regulation and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 353(1377), 1915-1927.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1-23.
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Voelker, P. (2014). Developing attention: Behavioral and brain mechanisms. Advances in Neuroscience, 2014, Article ID 405094. doi: 10.1155/2014/405094.
Ramey, S. L., & Ramey, C. T. (1998). Commentary: The transition to school opportunities and challenges for children, families, educators, and communities. Elementary School Journal, 98(4), 293-295.
Ray, W. J., & Cole, H. W. (1985). EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. Science, 228(4700), 750-752.
Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., & Weiskrantz, L. E. (1998). The prefrontal cortex: Executive and cognitive functions. Oxford University Press.
Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Temperament, development, and personality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(4), 207-212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00505.x
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. A. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner, Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99-166). John Wiley & Sons.
Rothbart, M. K., & Jones, L. B. (1998). Temperament, self-regulation, and education. School Psychology Review, 27(4), 479-491.
Rothbart, M. K., & Rueda, M. R. (2005). The development of effortful control. In U. Mayr, E. Awh, & S. Keele (Eds.), Developing individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael Posner (pp. 167–188). American Psychological Association.
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72(5), 1394-1408.
Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (1994). A psychobiological approach to the development of temperament. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual differences at the interface of biology and behavior (pp. 83–116). American Psychological Association.
Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., & Posner, M. I. (2011). Temperament and self-regulation. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 441-460). New York, US: Guilford Press
Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. Journal of Personality, 71(6), 1113-1144.
Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Kieras, J. (2006). Temperament, attention, and the development of self-regulation. In: K. McCartney, D. Phillips (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp.338-357).Blackwell.
Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2011). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early life. Emotion Review, 3(2), 207-213.
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029-1040.
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029-1040.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Attentional control and self-regulation. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 284–299). Guilford Press.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(41), 14931-14936.
Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early development: Themes and variations.Oxford University Press.
Ruff, H. A., Capozzoli, M.,&Weissberg, R. (1998). Age, individuality, and context as factor in sustained visual attention duration the preschool years. Development Psychology, 34(3), 454-464
Rush, B. R., Hafen, M., Jr., Biller, D. S., Davis, E. G., Klimek, J. A., Kukanich, B., Larson, R. L., Roush, J. K., Schermerhorn, T., Wilkerson, M. J., & White, B. J. (2010). The effect of differing audience response system question types on student attention in the veterinary medical classroom. Journal of Veterinary Medical Education, 37(2), 145-153.
Savage, R., Cornish, K., Manly, T., & Hollis, C. (2006). Cognitive processes in children's reading and attention: The role of working memory, divided attention, and response inhibition. British Journal of Psychology, 97(3), 365-385.
Savage, R., Cornish, K., Manly, T., & Hollis, C. (2006). Cognitive processes in children's reading and attention: The role of working memory, divided attention, and response inhibition. British Journal of Psychology, 97(3), 365-385.
Sheese, B. E., Voelker, P. M., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Parenting quality interacts with genetic variation in dopamine receptor D4 to influence temperament in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 19(4), 1039-1046.
Sheese, B. E., Voelker, P., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2009). Genetic variation influences on the early development of reactive emotions and their regulation by attention. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 14(4-5), 332-355.
Simonds, J., Kieras, J. E., Rueda, M. R., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Effortful control, executive attention, and emotional regulation in 7–10-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 22(4), 474-488.
Snyder, H. R., Miyake, A., & Hankin, B. L. (2015). Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: Bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 328.
Sternberg, R. J., & Sternberg, K. (2016). Cognitive psychology.Nelson Education.
Sulik, M. J., Huerta, S., Zerr, A. A., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., & Edwards, A. (2010). The factor structure of effortful control and measurement invariance across ethnicity and sex in a high-risk sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32(1), 8-22.
Tanaka, M., Ishii, A., & Watanabe, Y. (2014). Neural effects of mental fatigue caused by continuous attention load: a magnetoencephalography study. Brain Research, 1561, 60-66.
Tang, Y. Y., & Posner, M. I. (2009). Attention training and attention state training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(5), 222-227.
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. Brunner/Mazel.
Tiego, J., Bellgrove, M. A., Whittle, S., Pantelis, C., & Testa, R. (2019). Common mechanisms of executive attention underlie executive function and effortful control in children. Developmental Science (October), 1-25.
Vecchiato, G., Toppi, J., Astolfi, L., Fallani, F. D. V., Cincotti, F., Mattia, D., Bez, F., & Babiloni, F. (2011). Spectral EEG frontal asymmetries correlate with the experienced pleasantness of TV commercial advertisements. Medical & Biological Engineering & Computing, 49(5), 579-583.
White, L. K., McDermott, J. M., Degnan, K. A., Henderson, H. A., & Fox, N. A. (2011). Behavioral inhibition and anxiety: The moderating roles of inhibitory control and attention shifting. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(5), 735-747.
Zoefel, B., Huster, R. J., & Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Neurofeedback training of the upper alpha frequency band in EEG improves cognitive performance. Neuroimage, 54(2), 1427-1431.