簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 林佳慧
Lin, Chia-Hui
論文名稱: 學步兒氣質與母親教養雙向影響歷程及其對早期自我調節原型之影響
Transactional relationships between toddlers’ temperament and maternal parenting, and their associations with emerging self-regulation
指導教授: 張鑑如
Chang, Chien-Ju
口試委員: 林慧麗 涂妙如 邱皓政 程景琳 周麗端 張鑑如
口試日期: 2022/01/11
學位類別: 博士
Doctor
系所名稱: 人類發展與家庭學系
Department of Human Development and Family Studies
論文出版年: 2022
畢業學年度: 110
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 148
中文關鍵詞: 自我調節雙向影響關係氣質教養行為幼兒臺灣幼兒發展調查資料庫
英文關鍵詞: children, Kids in Taiwan: National Longitudinal Study of Child Development & Care project, parenting, self-regulation, temperament, transactional relationship
研究方法: 次級資料分析縱貫性研究
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202200358
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:286下載:77
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 本研究旨在探討影響早期幼兒自我調節發展的因素。自我調節能力在進入生命第二年時開始快速萌發,且對幼兒未來的社會適應以及認知學習有重大影響,因此是幼兒發展的重要目標。過去的研究多探討母親的教養行為或親子關係的品質與幼兒自我調節間的關聯,本研究則基於發展系統理論之生物心理社會架構,同時探討幼兒氣質因素與母親教養因素對幼兒早期自我調節的影響。本研究並從雙向影響關係(transactional relationships)的觀點,探討幼兒氣質因素與母親教養因素間的雙向影響歷程。本研究的目的有二:其一為探討幼兒氣質(包含負向情緒性氣質與奮力控制氣質)與母親教養(包含正向的母親回應及負向的條件式管教)之間是否具有持續性的雙向影響關係。其二為探討幼兒氣質及母親教養對幼兒早期自我調節的影響。
    根據理論及過去相關文獻之結果,本研究提出假設一:幼兒氣質與母親教養間有互相影響的關係,且此雙向影響關係具有持續性。假設二:負向情緒性氣質會對母親教養行為有負面影響(即會減低正向回應與增加條件式管教),奮力控制氣質則會對母親教養行為有正面影響(即會增加正向回應與減低條件式管教)。相對的,母親回應會對幼兒氣質有正面影響(即減低負向情緒性氣質與增加奮力控制氣質),條件式管教則對幼兒氣質有負面影響(即增加負向情緒性氣質與減低奮力控制氣質)。假設三:幼兒負向情緒性氣質會對其早期自我調節發展有負面影響、奮力控制氣質則有助幼兒早期自我調節的發展;母親回應可提昇幼兒早期自我調節,母親條件式管教則不利幼兒的早期自我調節發展。
    根據上述目的,本研究分析臺灣幼兒發展調查資料庫之3月齡組幼兒資料,並針對5,267位幼兒母親在幼兒12月齡、18月齡及24月齡時所填答的問卷資料,進行隨機截距自我迴歸交叉延宕模型分析。變項的來源包含母親所填答的認知發展問卷、社會情緒發展問卷及家庭問卷。
    本研究獲得以下結果:
    一、在12月齡到24月齡的三個時間點中,母親正負向教養行為和幼兒的奮力控制氣質間均具有持續的雙向影響關係。母親的回應與幼兒的奮力控制氣質具有互相增強的雙向影響關係,但母親的條件式管教和幼兒的奮力控制氣質則具有相互消減的雙向影響關係。幼兒的負向情緒性氣質則未與母親的教養行為產生持續性的雙向影響關係。研究假設一及二僅在奮力控制氣質與母親教養行為的部分獲得支持。
    二、幼兒的奮力控制氣質和母親的回應與條件式管教會影響幼兒的早期自我調節發展。12月齡時的奮力控制氣質與母親回應均可正向預測24月齡時的自我調節,但12月齡時的母親條件式管教則負向預測24月齡時的自我調節。由於本研究未發現幼兒負向情緒性氣質與早期自我調節發展間之關聯,因此研究假設之三也僅在奮力控制氣質及母親教養行為的部分獲得支持。
    本研究之結果支持母親的教養行為與孩子的氣質間具有相互影響與被影響的關係,意即母親不只扮演提供孩子社會化影響的角色,也會受到孩子的氣質或特質的影響而改變教養行為。本結果支持幼兒在自己社會化的歷程中扮演主動角色。
    此外,本研究之結果顯示幼兒的奮力控制氣質對早期自我調節發展具有顯著的影響,但氣質也會受到母親教養行為的影響,顯示個體生物性特質也會受環境脈絡等社會經驗之形塑。母親可藉由提供溫暖回應等正向教養行為增進幼兒的奮力控制氣質,但幼兒的奮力控制氣質也會因母親的責罰管教等負向教養行為而消減。

    The present study aimed to investigate factors that influence children’s self-regulatory development. Self-regulation emerges and develops rapidly as infants enter their second year of life, and the capacity of self-regulation has broad implications on children’s social adjustment as well as academic achievement. Thus, the development of self-regulation is fundamental for young children. While much of previous research underlined links between maternal parenting behaviors or quality of parent-child interactions with children’s self-regulation, the current study examined both children’s temperaments and maternal parenting behaviors as influencing factors based on biopsychosocial theoretical perspective which deems children’s development as general systems. In addition, the current study proposed that there would be bidirectional and transactional relationships between children’s temperaments and maternal parenting practices. The current study has two goals: The first one is to investigate whether children’s negative emotionality as well as effortful control has continuous bidirectional and transactional relationships with maternal responsiveness and conditional discipline practices. The second goal is to explore the impact of children’s both temperaments and both maternal parenting behaviors on children’s early self-regulation.
    Based on theories and findings of past literature, the current study raised 3 hypotheses.
    Hypothesis 1. There would be bidirectional and transactional relationships between children’s temperaments and maternal parenting practices, and these relationships could be observed continuously.
    Hypothesis 2. Specifically, children’s temperamental negative emotionality would have negative impacts on maternal parenting and decrease maternal responsiveness while increase conditional discipline practices. In addition, children’s temperamental effortful control would have positive impact on maternal parenting and increase maternal responsiveness as well as decrease conditional discipline practices. In the same way, maternal responsiveness would have positive influences on children’s temperaments and foster effortful control while decrease negative emotionality. Furthermore, maternal conditional discipline practices would have negative influences and increase children’s negative emotionality and decrease effortful control.
    Hypothesis 3. Children’s negative emotionality would have negative impacts on development of self-regulation, while effortful control would foster its development. In respect of maternal parenting behaviors, maternal responsiveness would have positive impact while conditional discipline practices would exert negative influences on children’s self-regulation.
    With above hypotheses, the current study analyzed data of M3 cohort collected by Kids in Taiwan: National Longitudinal Study of Child Development & Care project (KIT). Information were provided by 5,267 mothers who responded to questionnaires when their children were aged 12-month, 18-month, and 24-month. Random-Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Modeling (RI-CLPM) via Mplus 8.4 was used to test the research hypotheses. Research variables were constructed from responses to cognitive development questionnaire, social-emotional development questionnaire, and family environment questionnaire.
    Following results were observed.
    1. Among 3 timepoints from 12M to 24M, continuous bidirectional relationships between maternal parenting behaviors and children’s effortful control were found. To be specific, maternal responsiveness and children’s effortful control had positive reciprocal relationships and would foster each other over time, while maternal conditional discipline practices and children’s effortful control would reduce each other continuously. Yet links between children’s negative emotionality and maternal parenting practices were not found. As such, the first and the second hypothesis were both partially supported.
    2. Children’s effortful control, maternal responsiveness and conditional discipline practices, all measured at 12M, significantly impacted children’s self-regulation which was measured at 24M. Specifically, both children’s effortful control and maternal responsiveness showed positive effects on self-regulation, while maternal conditional discipline practices had negative effects on development of self-reguation.
    Findings of current study indicate that maternal parenting behaviors and children’s temperaments have transactional relationships, i.e. they are both influencing and influenced by each other. This finding supports the claim that mothers are not only playing roles in children’s socialization by providing specific parenting practices, what they provide or how they provide are also influenced by children’s dispositional temperament, suggesting children’s active role in their own socialization processes.
    In addition, the current study reveals that children’s effortful control temperament has enormous impact on self-regulatory development, yet it can be influenced by maternal parenting behaviors. The results suggest that children’s biological-rooted traits can be shaped by social contexts. Moreover, this study indicates that mothers can foster children’s effortful control by providing positive practices such as responsiveness and emotional supporting. However, children’s effortful control would be harmed should mother use negative discipline practices.

    誌謝詞 i 中文摘要 iii Abstract v 目次 viii 表次 ix 圖次 x 第一章 緒論 1 第一節 研究動機與背景 1 第二節 研究目的與研究問題 12 第三節 名詞解釋 15 第二章 文獻探討 19 第一節 雙向影響關係的理論觀點 19 第二節 自我調節之概念內涵與發展 30 第三節 影響早期自我調節發展的因素 37 第四節 母親教養行為與幼兒氣質的雙向影響關係 49 第三章 研究方法 57 第一節 研究架構 57 第二節 研究對象 59 第三節 研究工具 62 第四節 資料分析 68 第四章 研究結果 71 第一節 初步分析 71 第二節 幼兒氣質與母親教養的雙向影響關係 83 第三節 幼兒氣質、母親教養對早期自我調節的影響 93 第四節 模型的複核效度檢驗 97 第五章 結果討論與結論建議 100 第一節 結果討論 100 第二節 結論與建議 107 參考文獻 110 附錄 132

    壹、中文部分
    王珮玲(2010)。難養育嬰兒母親對其嬰兒身心特性的覺知與詮釋。臺北市立教育大學學報,41(1),1-34。
    王珮玲(2014)。嬰幼兒氣質回顧與前瞻:1980-2011。應用心理研究,61,55-112。
    汪佩蓉(2016)。全面性發展遲緩兒童之母親互動行為、精熟動機和發展能力之雙向關係(未出版之博士論文)。國立臺灣大學,臺北市。
    邱皓政(2011)。結構方程模式:Lisrel / Simplis 原理與應用(二版)。雙葉書廊。
    邱皓政(2017)。多層次模式與縱貫資料分析:Mplus 8解析應用。五南出版。
    邱皓政(2020)。貝氏統計原理與應用。雙葉書廊。
    林惠雅(2000)。母親與幼兒互動中之教養行為分析。應用心理研究,6,75-96。
    吳齊殷、張明宜、陳怡蒨(2008)。尋找機制與過程:長期追蹤研究的功用。αβγ量化研究學刊,2(1),1-26。
    哈里斯(2011)。教養的迷思:父母的教養能不能決定孩子的人格發展?(洪蘭、蘇奕君譯)。商周出版。(原著出版年:1998/2009二版)
    黃世錚、李麗雯(2010)。母親教養行為與幼兒獨立完成作業關聯性之追蹤研究。教育心理學報,42(2),209-228。http://dx.doi.org/10.6251/BEP.20090527
    張鑑如、謝淑惠、周麗端、廖鳯瑞(2017)。幼兒發展調查資料庫建置計畫簡介。中國統計學報,55(1),42-62。
    鄭芬蘭(2000)。教育大辭書(雙語詞彙、學術名詞辭書資訊網),國家教育研究院。https://terms.naer.edu.tw/detail/1305296/?index=21
    潘韻如、王湉妮(2015)。親子關係雙向模式之介紹與實務應用。台灣職能治療研究與實務,11(2),100-108。https://doi.org/10.6534/jtotrp.2015.119(2).100。

    貳、外文部分
    Amicarelli, A. R., Kotelnikova, Y., Smith, H. J., Kryski, K. R., & Hayden, E. P. (2018). Parenting differentially influences the development of boys' and girls' inhibitory control. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(3), 371-383. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12220
    Augustine, M. E., Leerkes, E. M., Smolen, A., & Calkins, S. D. (2018). Relations between early maternal sensitivity and toddler self-regulation: Exploring variation by oxytocin and dopamine D2 receptor genes. Developmental Psychobiology, 60(7), 789-804. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21745
    Avinun, R. & Knafo-Noam, A. (2015). Socialization, genetics, and their interplay in development. In J. E. Grusec, & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 347 - 371). Guilford.
    Ayoub, C. C., & Fischer, K. W. (2006). Developmental pathways and intersections among domains of development. In K. McCartney & D. Phillips (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp.62-81). Blackwell Publishing.
    Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2006). Gene-environment interaction of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and observed maternal insensitivity predicting externalizing behavior in preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(5), 406-409. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20152
    Bandura, A. (1991). Social Cognitive Theory of Self-Regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 248-287. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90022-L
    Barnett, M. A., & Scaramella, L. V. (2015). Child fear reactivity and sex as moderators of links between parenting and preschool behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4), 1179-1190. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000759
    Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child Development, 37(4), 887-907. https://doi.org/10.2307/1126611
    Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology, 4(1 PART 2), 1-103. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030372
    Beaver, K. M., & Belsky, J. (2012). Gene-environment interaction and the intergenerational transmission of parenting: testing the differential-susceptibility hypothesis. Psychiatric Quarterly, 83(1), 29-40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11126-011-9180-4
    Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75(2), 81-95. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025583
    Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 300-304. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x
    Belsky, J., Fearon, R. M. P., & Bell, B. (2007). Parenting, attention and externalizing problems: Testing mediation longitudinally, repeatedly and reciprocally. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 48(12), 1233-1242. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01807.x
    Belsky, J., Hsieh, K. H., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys' externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Development and Psychopathology, 10(2), 301-319. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457949800162X
    Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental Influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885-908. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017376
    Belsky, J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2017). Genetic differential susceptibility to the effects of parenting. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 125-130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.021
    Bernier, A., Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. A. (2016). Longitudinal associations between the quality of mother–infant interactions and brain development across infancy. Child Development, 87(4), 1159-1174. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12518
    Birmingham, R. S., Bub, K. L., & Vaughn, B. E. (2017). Parenting in infancy and self-regulation in preschool: An investigation of the role of attachment history. Attachment and Human Development, 19(2), 107-129. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2016.1259335
    Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. The American psychologist, 57(2), 111-127. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.57.2.111
    Blair, C., & Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 899-911. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000436
    Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 66, 711-734. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221
    Bohr, Y., Putnick, D. L., Lee, Y., & Bornstein, M. H. (2018). Evaluating caregiver sensitivity to infants: Measures matter. Infancy, 23(5), 730-747. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12248
    Boldt, L. J., Goffin, K. C., & Kochanska, G. (2020). The significance of early parent-child attachment for emerging regulation: A longitudinal investigation of processes and mechanisms from toddler age to preadolescence. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 431-443. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000862
    Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Gartstein, M. A., Hahn, C. S., Auestad, N., & O'Connor, D. L. (2015). Infant temperament: Stability by age, gender, birth order, term status, and socioeconomic status. Child Development, 86(3), 844-863. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12367
    Brandes-Aitken, A., Braren, S., Swingler, M., Voegtline, K., & Blair, C. (2019). Sustained attention in infancy: A foundation for the development of multiple aspects of self-regulation for children in poverty. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 184, 192-209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.04.006
    Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M. M., & Stifter, C. A. (1997). Compliance and noncompliance: The roles of maternal control and child temperament. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18(3), 411-428. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(97)80008-1
    Bridgett, D. J., Gartstein, M. A., Putnam, S. P., McKay, T., Iddins, E., Robertson, C., Ramsay, K., & Rittmueller, A. (2009). Maternal and contextual influences and the effect of temperament development during infancy on parenting in toddlerhood. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 103-116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.007
    Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101(4), 568-586. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.568
    Bronfenbrenner, U., & Evans, G. W. (2000). Developmental science in the 21st century: Emerging questions, theoretical models, research designs and empirical findings. Social Development, 9(1), 115-125. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00114
    Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 793-828). John Wiley & Sons.
    Bronson, M. B. (2000). Self-regulation in early childhood: Nature and Nurture. Guilford.
    Brownell, C. A., & Kopp, C. B. (2007). Transitions in toddler socioemotional development: Behavior, understanding, relationships. In C. A. Brownell & C. B. Kopp (Eds.), Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations (pp. 1-40). Guilford.
    Buss, K. A., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child Development, 69(2), 359-374. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06195.x
    Byrne, B. M. (2012). Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Routledge.
    Calkins, S. D. (2007). The emergence of self-regulation: Biological and behavioral control mechanisms supporting toddler competencies. In C. A. Brownell & C. B. Kopp (Eds.), Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations (pp. 261-284). Guilford.
    Calkins, S. D. (2015). Introduction to the volume: Seeing infant development through a biopsychosocial lens. In S. D. Calkins (Ed.), Handbook of infant biopsychosocial development (pp. 3-10). Guilford.
    Calkins, S. D., Blandon, A. Y., Williford, A. P., & Keane, S. P. (2007). Biological, behavioral, and relational levels of resilience in the context of risk for early childhood behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 19(3), 675-700. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940700034X
    Calkins, S. D., Dedmon, S. E., Gill, K. L., Lomax, L. E., & Johnson, L. M. (2002). Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion regulation, physiological processes, and temperament. Infancy, 3(2), 175-197. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0302_4
    Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: A multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 14(3), 477-498. https://doi.org/10.1017.S0954579402003057
    Calkins, S. D., Hungerford, A., & Dedmon, S. E. (2004). Mothers' interactions with temperamentally frustrated infants [Article]. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25(3), 219-239. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20002
    Calkins, S. D., Perry, N. B., & Dollar, J. M. (2016). A biopsychosocial model of self-regulation in infancy. In L. Balter & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child Psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (3rd ed., pp. 3-20). Routledge.
    Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral and physiological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development, 7(3), 350-369. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00072
    Caspi, A., McCray, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., Taylor, A., & Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297(5582), 851-854. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1072290
    Cassidy, J. (2016). The nature of the child’s ties. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed., pp. 3-24). Guilford.
    Cha, K. (2017). Relationships among negative emotionality, responsive parenting and early socio-cognitive development in Korean children. Infant and Child Development, 26(3), Article e1990. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1990
    Chang, H., Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., & Sexton, H. R. (2011). Child effortful control as a mediator of parenting practices on externalizing behavior: Evidence for a sex-differentiated pathway across the transition from preschool to school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(1), 71-81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9437-7
    Chen, N., Deater-Deckard, K., & Bell, M. A. (2014). The Role of temperament by family environment interactions in child maladjustment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(8), 1251-1262. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9872-y
    Cheung, G. W. (2008). Testing equivalence in the structure, means, and variances of higher-order constructs with structural equation modeling. Organizational Research Methods, 11(3), 593-613. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428106298973
    Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit Indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9(2), 233-255. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem0902_5
    Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(4), 597-600. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400007318
    Cole, P. M., Ram, N., & English, M. S. (2019). Toward a unifying model of self-regulation: A developmental approach. Child Development Perspectives, 13(2), 91-96. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12316
    Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55(2), 218-232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.2.218
    Colman, R. A., Hardy, S. A., Albert, M., Raffaelli, M., & Crockett, L. (2006). Early predictors of self-regulation in middle childhood. Infant and Child Development, 15(4), 421-437. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.469
    Combs-Ronto, L. A., Olson, S. L., Lunkenheimer, E. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (2009). Interactions between maternal parenting and children's early disruptive behavior: Bidirectional associations across the transition from preschool to school entry. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(8), 1151-1163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9332-2
    Cruz, O., Abreu-Lima, I., Canário, C., & Burchinal, M. (2018). Does temperament moderate the relation between preschool parenting and school-age self-regulation? Contrasting diathesis-stress and differential susceptibility models. Parenting, 18(2), 126-140. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2018.1444133
    Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An integrative model. Psychological Bulletin, 113(3), 487-496. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.487
    Deater-Deckard, K., & Cahill, K. (2006). Nature and nurture in early childhood. In K. McCartney & D. Phillips (Eds.,), Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp.3-21). Blackwell.
    Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., & Spinrad, T. L. (2011). Effortful control. In K. Vohs & R. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 263-283). Guilford.
    Eisenberg, N., Taylor, Z. E., Widaman, K. F., & Spinrad, T. L. (2015). Externalizing symptoms, effortful control, and intrusive parenting: A test of bidirectional longitudinal relations during early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4), 953-968. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000620
    Eisenberg, N., & Valiente, C. (2012). Parenting and children’s prosocial and moral development. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 5. Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed., pp. 111-142). Psychology Press.
    Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children's effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76(5), 1055-1071. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00897.x
    Elliott, J., Holland, J., & Thomas, R. (2008). Longitudinal and panel studies. In P. Alasuutari, L. Bickman, & J. Brannen (Eds.), The Sage handbook of social research methods (pp. 1-29). Sage.
    Feng, X., Hooper, E. G., & Jia, R. (2017). From compliance to self-regulation: Development during early childhood. Social Development, 26(4), 981-995. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12245
    Gartstein, M. A., Bridgett, D. J., Young, B. N., Panksepp, J., & Power, T. (2013). Origins of effortful control: Infant and parent contributions. Infancy, 18(2), 149-183. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00119.x
    Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 26(1), 64-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00169-8
    Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1987). Roundtable: What is temperament? Four approaches. Child Development, 58(2), 505-529. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3829791
    Gonzalez, R. (2009). Transactions and statistical modeling: Developmental theory wagging the statistical tail. In A. Sameroff (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other (pp. 223-245). American Psychological Association.
    Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 4-13.
    Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10(1), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00556.x
    Graziano, P. A., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2010). Maternal behavior and children's early emotion regulation skills differentially predict development of children's reactive control and later effortful control. Infant and Child Development, 19(4), 333-353. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.670
    Grolnick, W. S., & Farkas, M. (2012). Parenting and the development of children's self-regulation. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 5. Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed., pp. 89-110). Psychology.
    Gross, J. J. & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3-24). Guilford.
    Grusec, J. E. (1997). A history of research on parenting strategies and children’s internalization of values. In J. E. Grusec, & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and children’s internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 3-22). John Wiley & Sons.
    Grusec, J. E. (2011). Socialization processes in the family: Social and emotional development. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 243-269. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131650
    Grusec, J. E. (2019). Principles of effective parenting: How socialization works. Guilford.
    Grusec, J. E., & Davidov, M. (2010). Integrating different perspectives on socialization theory and research: A domain-specific approach. Child Development, 81(3), 687-709. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01426.x
    Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. P. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 102-116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889
    Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102(3), 458-489.
    Hoyle, R. H. (2014). Personality and self-regulation. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of personality and self-regulation (pp.1-18). Wiley Blackwell.
    Hughes, C., Devine, R. T., Mesman, J., & Blair, C. (2020). Understanding the terrible twos: A longitudinal investigation of the impact of early executive function and parent–child interactions. Developmental Science, 23(6), e12979. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12979
    Institute of Medicine & National Research Council. (2015). Transforming the workforce for children birth through age 8: A unifying foundation. National Academies Press.
    Karoly, P. (1993). Mechanisms of self-regulation: A systems view. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 23-52.
    Karreman, A., van Tuijl, C., van Aken, M. A. G., & Deković, M. (2006). Parenting and self-regulation in preschoolers: A meta-analysis. Infant and Child Development, 15(6), 561-579. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.478
    Kiff, C. J., Lengua, L. J., & Zalewski, M. (2011). Nature and nurturing: Parenting in the context of child temperament. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14(3), 251-301. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0093-4
    Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2012). Child temperament moderates effects of parent-child mutuality on self-regulation: A relationship-based path for emotionally negative Infants. Child Development, 83(4), 1275-1289. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01778.x
    Klein, M. R., Lengua, L. J., Thompson, S. F., Moran, L., Ruberry, E. J., Kiff, C., & Zalewski, M. (2018). Bidirectional relations between temperament and parenting predicting preschool-age children's adjustment. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 47(sup1), S113-S126. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2016.1169537
    Kochanska, G. (1991). Socialization and temperament in the development of guilt and conscience. Child Development, 62(6), 1379-1392.
    Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64(2), 325-347.
    Kochanska, G., Boldt, L. J., & Goffin, K. C. (2019). Early relational experience: A foundation for the unfolding dynamics of parent–child socialization. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 41-47. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12308
    Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72(4), 1091-1111. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00336
    Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2020). Children's early difficulty and agreeableness in adolescence: Testing a developmental model of interplay of parent and child effects. Developmental Psychology, 56(8), 1556-1564. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001023
    Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71(6), 1087-1112. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.7106008
    Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220-232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220
    Koole, S. L., van Dillen, L. F., & Sheppes, G. (2011). The self-regulation of emotion. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulaton: Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 22-40). Guilford.
    Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 199-214. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.18.2.199
    Kuczynski, L. (2003). Beyond bidirectionality: Bilateral conceptual frameworks for understanding dynamics in parent-child relations. In L. Kuczynski (Ed.), Handbook of dynamics in parent-child relations (pp. 3-24). Sage.
    Kuczynski, L., & de Mol, J. (2015). Dialectical models of socialization. In W. F. Overton & P. C. M. Molenaar (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Vol. 1. Theory and method (7th ed., pp. 323-368). John Wiley & Sons.
    Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2015). The role of parent-child relationships in child development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (7th ed., pp.535-586). Psychology Press.
    Lerner, R. M., Hershberg, R. M., Hilliard, L. J., & Johnson, S. K. (2015). Concepts and theories of human development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (7th ed., pp.3-41). Psychology Press.
    Lerner, R. M., & Overton, W. F. (2008). Exemplifying the integrations of the relational developmental system: Synthesizing theory, research, and application to promote positive development and social justice. Journal of Adolescent Research, 23(3), 245-255. https://doi.org/10.1177/0743558408314385
    Liu, P., Kryski, K. R., Smith, H. J., Joanisse, M. F., & Hayden, E. P. (2020). Transactional relations between early child temperament, structured parenting, and child outcomes: A three-wave longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 32(3), 923-933. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000841
    Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The Role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview. Developmental Psychology, 28(6), 1006-1017. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1006
    Maccoby, E. E. (2000). Parenting and its effects on children: On reading and misreading behavior genetics. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 1-27. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.1
    Maccoby, E. E. (2015). Historical overview of socialization research and theory. In J. E. Grusec, & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 3-32). Guilford.
    Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed., pp. 1-101). John Wiley & Sons.
    Magnusson, D. (2000). The individual as organizing principle in psychological inquiry: A holistic approach. In L. R. Bergman, R. B. Cairns, L.-G. Nilsson, & L. Nystedt (Eds.), Developmental science and the holistic approach (pp. 33-47). Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491-495. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222
    McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2011). Self-regulation and academic achievement in elementary school children. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2011(133), 29-44. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.302
    McClelland, M., Geldhof, J., Morrison, F., Gestsdóttir, S., Cameron, C., Bowers, E., Duckworth, A., Little, T., & Grammar, J. (2018). Self-regulation. In N. Halfon, C. B. Forrest, R. M. Lerner, & E. M. Faustman (Eds.), Handbook of Life Course Health Development (pp. 275-298). Springer.
    McDermott, J. M., & Fox, N. A. (2014). Exploring response monitoring: Developmental differences and contributions to self-regulation. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of personality and self-regulation (pp. 91-113). Wiley Blackwell.
    Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp.99-129). Guilford.
    Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(2), 329-337.
    Mulder, J. D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2021). Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28(4), 638-648. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738
    Muthén, K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2019). Mplus version 8.4. Los Angeles, CA: Author.
    Napolitano, C. M., & Freund, A. M. (2019). Adding life to one’s added years: Self- regulatory balancing of life domains across old age. Advances in Life Course Research, 41, [100278]. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.alcr.2019.04.008.
    Neppl, T. K., Jeon, S., Diggs, O., & Donnellan, M. B. (2020). Positive parenting, effortful control, and developmental outcomes across early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 444-457. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000874
    Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3-4), 395-422. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x
    Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual research review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361-383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12675
    Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C. R. R., Lopez, N. L., & Wellman, H. M. (2005). Developmental foundations of externalizing problems in young children: The role of effortful control. Development and Psychopathology, 17(1), 25-45. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050029
    Overton, W. F. (2013). A new paradigm for developmental science: Relationism and relational-developmental systems. Applied Developmental Science, 17(2), 94-107. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2013.778717
    Overton, W. F. (2015). Processes, relations, and relational-developmental-systems. In W. F. Overton & P. Molenaar (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Vol. 1. Theory and method (7th ed., pp. 9-62). John Wiley & Sons.
    Pardini, D. A. (2008). Novel insights into longstanding theories of bidirectional parent-child influences: Introduction to the special section. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(5), 627-631. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9231-y
    Perry, N. B., Calkins, S. D., Dollar, J. M., Keane, S. P., & Shanahan, L. (2018). Self-regulation as a predictor of patterns of change in externalizing behaviors from infancy to adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 30(2), 497-510. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417000992
    Perry, N. B., Dollar, J. M., Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. A. (2018). Developmental cascade and transactional associations among biological and behavioral indicators of temperament and maternal behavior. Child Development, 89(5), 1735-1751. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12842
    Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 427-441. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400003096
    Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2018). Temperament and brain networks of attention. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 373(1744), 20170254. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0254
    Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Tang, Y. Y. (2015). Enhancing attention through training. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 4, 1-5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.12.008
    Robson, D. A., Allen, M. S., & Howard, S. J. (2020). Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 146(4), 324-354. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000227
    Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52(2), 569-578. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1981.tb03082.x
    Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72(5), 1394-1408. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00355
    Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99-166). John Wiley & Sons.
    Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. Journal of Personality, 71(6), 1113-1144. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.7106009
    Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Kieras, J. (2006). Temperament, attention, and the development of Self-Regulation. In K. McCartney & D. Phillips (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp. 338-357). Blackwell Publishing.
    Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2011). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early life. Emotion Review, 3(2), 207-213. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910387943
    Rueda, M. R. (2012). Effortful control. In M. Zentner & R. L. Shiner (Eds.). Handbook of temperament (pp. 145-167). Guilford.
    Sameroff, A. (1975). Transactional models in early social relations. Human Development, 18(1-2), 65-79. https://doi.org/10.1159/000271476
    Sameroff, A. (2009). The transactional model. In A. Sameroff (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other (pp.3-21). American Psychological Association.
    Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: A dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Development, 81(1), 6-22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01378.x
    Scaramella, L. V., Sohr-Preston, S. L., Mirabile, S. P., Robison, S. D., & Callahan, K. L. (2008). Parenting and children's distress reactivity during toddlerhood: An examination of direction of effects. Social Development, 17(3), 578-595. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00439.x
    Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development, 63(1), 1-19.
    Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype greater than environment effects. Child Development, 54(2), 424-435. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1983.tb03884.x
    Shiner, R. L., Buss, K. A., McClowry, S. G., Putnam, S. P., Saudino, K. J., & Zentner, M. (2012). What is temperament now? Assessing progress temperament research on the twenty-fifth anniversary of Goldsmith et al. (1987). Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 436-444. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00254.x
    Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early child development. National Academy Press.
    Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Fabes, R. A., Valiente, C., Shepard, S. A., Reiser, M., Losoya, S. H., & Guthrie, I. K. (2006). Relation of emotion-related regulation to children's social competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion, 6(3), 498-510. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.498
    Sroufe, L. A. (2016). The place of attachment in development. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed., pp. 997-1011). Guilford.
    Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota Study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. Guilford.
    Strelau, J., & Angleitner, A. (1991). Introduction - Temperament research: Some divergences and similarities. In J. Strelau & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on theory and measurement (pp.1-12). Plenum.
    The Caroline Consortium on Human Development. (1996). Developmental Science: A collaborative statement. In R. B. Cairns, G. H. Elder, Jr., & E. J. Costello (Eds.), Developmental Science (pp. 1-6). Cambridge University Press.
    Thompson, M. J., Davies, P. T., Hentges, R. F., Sturge-Apple, M. L., & Parry, L. Q. (2020). Understanding how and why effortful control moderates children's vulnerability to interparental conflict. Developmental Psychology, 56(5), 937-950. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000909
    Ursache, A., Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). The promotion of self-regulation as a means of enhancing school readiness and early achievement in children at risk for school failure. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 122-128. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00209.x
    Usami, S. (2021). On the differences between general cross-lagged panel model and random-intercept cross-lagged panel model: Interpretation of cross-lagged parameters and model choice. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28(3), 331-344. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1821690
    Usami, S., Murayama, K., & Hamaker, E. L. (2019). A unified framework of longitudinal models to examine reciprocal relations. Psychological Methods, 24(5), 637-657. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000210
    Witherington, D. C. (2018). Dynamic systems theory. In A. S. Dick & U. Müller (Eds.), Advancing developmental science: Philosophy, theory, and method (pp. 41-52). Routledge.
    Wittig, S. M. O., & Rodriguez, C. M. (2019). Emerging behavior problems: Bidirectional relations between maternal and paternal parenting styles with infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 55(6), 1199-1210. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000707
    Xu, J., Zhang, Q., & Yang, Y. (2020). Impact of violations of measurement invariance in cross-lagged panel mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, 52(6), 2623-2645. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01426-z
    Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2018). Embracing complexity in the study of executive function and its development. In A. S. Dick & U. Müller (Eds.), Advancing developmental science: Philosophy, theory, and method (pp. 110-118). Routledge.
    Zhang, X., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Fosco, G. M., & Bierman, K. L. (2020). Parental support of self-regulation among children at risk for externalizing symptoms: Developmental trajectories of physiological regulation and behavioral adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 528-540. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000794
    Zhou, Q., Chen, S. H., & Main, A. (2012). Commonalities and differences in the research on children's effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self-regulation. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 112-121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00176.x

    下載圖示
    QR CODE